Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

The (possible) Cognitive Naturalness of Witchcraft Beliefs: An Exploration of the Existing Literature

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Journal of Cognition and Culture

Cross-culturally, misfortune is often attributed to witchcraft despite the high human and social costs of these beliefs. The evolved cognitive features that are often used to explain religion more broadly, in combination with threat perception and coalitional psychology, may help explain why these particular supernatural beliefs are so prevalent. Witches are minimally counter intuitive, agentic, and build upon intuitive understandings of ritual efficacy. Witchcraft beliefs may gain traction in threatening contexts and because they are threatening themselves, while simultaneously activating coalitional reasoning systems that make rejection of the idea costly. This article draws possible connections between these cognitive and environmental features with an eye toward future empirical examination.

Affiliations: 1: University of Lyon LyonFrance nora.parren@gmail.com

10.1163/15685373-12340015
/content/journals/10.1163/15685373-12340015
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
10
5
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/15685373-12340015
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Adinkrah M. 2011"Child witch hunts in contemporary Ghana" Child abuse & neglectVol 35 9 741 752 [Crossref]
2. Altshteyn I. 2014"Evidence for a warning bias in information transmission in social networks"
3. Ashforth A. 2002"An epidemic of witchcraft? The implications of aids for the post-apartheid state" African StudiesVol 61 1 121 143 [Crossref]
4. Barrett J. L. 2004 Why would anyone believe in God? AltaMira Press
5. Barrett J. L., Lanman J. A. 2008"The science of religious beliefs" ReligionVol 38 2 109 124 [Crossref]
6. Barrett J. L., Lawson E. T. 2001"Ritual intuitions: Cognitive contributions to judgments of ritual efficacy" Journal of cognition and cultureVol 1 2 183 201 [Crossref]
7. Baumeister R. F., Bratslavsky E., Finkenauer C., Vohs K. D. 2001"Bad is stronger than good" Review of general psychologyVol 5 4 323 [Crossref]
8. Baumeister R. F., Leary M. R. 1995"The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation" Psychological bulletinVol 117 3 497 [Crossref]
9. Bebbington K., MacLeod C., Ellison T. M., Fay N. 2017"The sky is falling: evidence of a negativity bias in the social transmission of information" Evolution and Human BehaviorVol 38 1 92 101 [Crossref]
10. Behringer W. 2004 Witches and witch-hunts: A global history Polity
11. Berger J., Milkman K. L. 2012"What makes online content viral?" Journal of marketing researchVol 49 2 192 205 [Crossref]
12. Bigler R. S., Jones L. C., Lobliner D. B. 1997"Social categorization and the formation of intergroup attitudes in children" Child developmentVol 68 3 530 543 [Crossref]
13. Blumberg J. 2007, 2011"A Brief History of the Salem Witch Trials" from http://www.smithsonianmag.com/history/a-brief-history-of-the-salem-witch-trials-175162489/
14. Boyer P. 1996"What makes anthropomorphism natural: Intuitive ontology and cultural representations" Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 83 97 [Crossref]
15. Boyer P. 2003"Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function" Trends in cognitive sciencesVol 7 3 119 124 [Crossref]
16. Boyer P. In Prep"Why Do People Try To Explain Misfortune? and, Why do it in terms of agents?"
17. Boyer P., Bergstrom B. 2011"Threat-detection in child development: An evolutionary perspective" Neuroscience & Biobehavioral ReviewsVol 35 4 1034 1041 [Crossref]
18. Boyer P., Parren N. 2015"Threat-related information suggests competence: a possible factor in the spread of rumors" PloS oneVol 10 6 e0128421 [Crossref]
19. Boyer P., Ramble C. 2001"Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations" Cognitive ScienceVol 25 4 535 564 [Crossref]
20. Buss D. M., Kenrick D. T. 1998"Evolutionary social psychology"
21. Butler A. C., Kang S. H., Roediger H. L. III 2009"Congruity effects between materials and processing tasks in the survival processing paradigm" Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and CognitionVol 35 6 1477 [Crossref]
22. Callan A. 2007"‘What else do we Bengalis do?’ Sorcery, overseas migration, and the new inequalities in Sylhet, Bangladesh" Journal of the Royal Anthropological InstituteVol 13 2 331 343 [Crossref]
23. Cosmides L., Tooby J. 2005"Neurocognitive adaptations designed for social exchange" The handbook of evolutionary psychology 584 627
24. Cosmides L., Tooby J., Fiddick L., Bryant G. A. 2005"Detecting cheaters" Trends in cognitive sciencesVol 9 505 506 [Crossref]
25. Crawford M. T., Sherman S. J., Hamilton D. L. 2002"Perceived entitativity, stereotype formation, and the interchangeability of group members" Journal of personality and social psychologyVol 83 5 1076 [Crossref]
26. Douglas M. 2013 Witchcraft confessions and accusations Routledge
27. Eisenberger N. I., Lieberman M. D., Williams K. D. 2003"Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion" ScienceVol 302 5643 290 292 [Crossref]
28. Epley N., Waytz A., Akalis S., Cacioppo J. T. 2008"When we need a human: Motivational determinants of anthropomorphism" Social cognitionVol 26 2 143 155 [Crossref]
29. Eriksson K., Coultas J. C. 2014"Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats: Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of urban legends" Journal of cognition and cultureVol 14 1–2 1 26 [Crossref]
30. Esses V. M., Dovidio J. F., Jackson L. M., Armstrong T. L. 2001"The immigration dilemma: The role of perceived group competition, ethnic prejudice, and national identity" Journal of Social issuesVol 57 3 389 412 [Crossref]
31. Evans-Pritchard E. E. 1937 Witchcraft, magic and oracles among the Azande Oxford Clarendon Press
32. Fandrich I. J. 2007"Yorùbá influences on Haitian vodou and New Orleans voodoo" Journal of Black StudiesVol 37 5 775 791 [Crossref]
33. Faulkner J., Schaller M., Park J. H., Duncan L. A. 2004"Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes" Group Processes & Intergroup RelationsVol 7 4 333 353 [Crossref]
34. Fessler D. M. 2006"Steps toward an evolutionary psychology of a culture-dependent species"
35. Fessler D. M., Holbrook C., Dashoff D. 2016"Dressed to kill? Visible markers of coalitional affiliation enhance conceptualized formidability" Aggressive behavior
36. Fessler D. M., Pisor A. C., Holbrook C. 2017"Political orientation predicts credulity regarding putative hazards" Psychological Science
37. Fessler D. M., Pisor A. C., Navarrete C. D. 2014"Negatively-biased credulity and the cultural evolution of beliefs" PloS oneVol 9 4 e95167 [Crossref]
38. Fu F., Tarnita C. E., Christakis N. A., Wang L., Rand D. G., Nowak M. A. 2012"Evolution of in-group favoritism" Scientific reportsVol 2 460 [Crossref]
39. Gershman B. 2016"Witchcraft beliefs and the erosion of social capital: Evidence from Sub-Saharan Africa and beyond" Journal of Development EconomicsVol 120 182 208 [Crossref]
40. Goette L., Huffman D., Meier S. 2006"The impact of group membership on cooperation and norm enforcement: Evidence using random assignment to real social groups"
41. Guthrie S. E. 1995 Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion Oxford University Press
42. Hayes K. E. 2007"Black magic and the academy: macumba and Afro-Brazilian “orthodoxies”" History of ReligionsVol 46 4 283 315 [Crossref]
43. Heath C. 1996"Do people prefer to pass along good or bad news? Valence and relevance of news as predictors of transmission propensity" Organizational behavior and human decision processesVol 68 2 79 94 [Crossref]
44. Heath C., Bell C., Sternberg E. 2001"Emotional selection in memes: the case of urban legends" Journal of personality and social psychologyVol 81 6 1028 [Crossref]
45. Hilbig B. E. 2009"Sad, thus true: Negativity bias in judgments of truth" Journal of Experimental Social PsychologyVol 45 4 983 986 [Crossref]
46. Hilbig B. E. 2012"How framing statistical statements affects subjective veracity: Validation and application of a multinomial model for judgments of truth" CognitionVol 125 1 37 48 [Crossref]
47. Kahneman D., Tversky A. 1984"Choices, values, and frames" American psychologistVol 39 4 341 [Crossref]
48. Kang S. H., McDermott K. B., Cohen S. M. 2008"The mnemonic advantage of processing fitness-relevant information" Memory & CognitionVol 36 6 1151 1156 [Crossref]
49. Kazanas S. A., Altarriba J. 2017"Did our ancestors fear the unknown? The role of predation in the survival advantage" Evolutionary Behavioral SciencesVol 11 1 83 [Crossref]
50. Kidder T. 2003 Mountains beyond mountains: healing the world: the quest of Dr. Paul Farmer New York Random House
51. Koning N. 2013"Witchcraft beliefs and witch hunts" Human natureVol 24 2 158 181 [Crossref]
52. Kurzban R., Leary M. R. 2001"Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: the functions of social exclusion" Psychological bulletinVol 127 2 187 [Crossref]
53. Kurzban R., Tooby J., Cosmides L. 2001"Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization" Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesVol 98 26 15387 15392 [Crossref]
54. Legare C. H., Gelman S. A. 2008"Bewitchment, biology, or both: The co‐existence of natural and supernatural explanatory frameworks across development" Cognitive ScienceVol 32 4 607 642 [Crossref]
55. Legare C. H., Souza A. L. 2012"Evaluating ritual efficacy: Evidence from the supernatural" CognitionVol 124 1 1 15 [Crossref]
56. Lickel B., Miller N., Stenstrom D. M., Denson T. F., Schmader T. 2006"Vicarious retribution: The role of collective blame in intergroup aggression" Personality and social psychology reviewVol 10 4 372 390 [Crossref]
57. Liénard P., Boyer P. 2006"Whence collective rituals? A cultural selection model of ritualized behavior" American AnthropologistVol 108 4 814 827 [Crossref]
58. Lienard P., Lawson E. T. 2008"Evoked culture, ritualization and religious rituals" ReligionVol 38 2 157 171 [Crossref]
59. Lisdorf A. 2007"What’s hidd’n in the hadd?" Journal of cognition and cultureVol 7 3 341 353 [Crossref]
60. McCauley R. N., Lawson E. T. 2002 Bringing ritual to mind: Psychological foundations of cultural forms Cambridge University Press [Crossref]
61. Mesoudi A., Whiten A., Dunbar R. 2006"A bias for social information in human cultural transmission" British Journal of PsychologyVol 97 3 405 423 [Crossref]
62. Miguel E. 2005"Poverty and witch killing" The Review of Economic StudiesVol 72 4 1153 1172 [Crossref]
63. Morewedge C. K. 2009"Negativity bias in attribution of external agency" Journal of Experimental Psychology: GeneralVol 138 4 535 [Crossref]
64. Nairne J. S., Pandeirada J. N. 2008"Adaptive memory: Is survival processing special?" Journal of Memory and LanguageVol 59 3 377 385 [Crossref]
65. Nairne J. S., Pandeirada J. N. 2010"Adaptive memory: Ancestral priorities and the mnemonic value of survival processing" Cognitive psychologyVol 61 1 1 22 [Crossref]
66. Nairne J. S., Thompson S. R., Pandeirada J. N. 2007"Adaptive memory: survival processing enhances retention" Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and CognitionVol 33 2 263 [Crossref]
67. Navarrete C. D., Kurzban R., Fessler D. M., Kirkpatrick L. A. 2004"Anxiety and intergroup bias: Terror management or coalitional psychology?" Group Processes & Intergroup RelationsVol 7 4 370 397 [Crossref]
68. Navarrete C. D., McDonald M. M., Asher B. D., Kerr N. L., Yokota K., Olsson A., Sidanius J. 2012"Fear is readily associated with an out-group face in a minimal group context" Evolution and Human BehaviorVol 33 5 590 593 [Crossref]
69. Olds J. M., Lanska M., Westerman D. L. 2014"The role of perceived threat in the survival processing memory advantage" MemoryVol 22 1 26 35 [Crossref]
70. Olsson A., Ebert J. P., Banaji M. R., Phelps E. A. 2005"The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear" ScienceVol 309 5735 785 787 [Crossref]
71. Parish J. 2011"West African witchcraft, wealth and moral decay in New York City" EthnographyVol 12 2 247 265 [Crossref]
72. Parish J. 2013"Chasing Celebrity: Akan Witchcraft and New York City" EthnosVol 78 2 280 300 [Crossref]
73. Peters K., Kashima Y., Clark A. 2009"Talking about others: Emotionality and the dissemination of social information" European Journal of Social PsychologyVol 39 2 207 222 [Crossref]
74. Pietraszewski D. 2013"What is group psychology? Adaptations for mapping shared intentional stances" Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us 253 257 [Crossref]
75. Pietraszewski D., Curry O. S., Petersen M. B., Cosmides L., Tooby J. 2015"Constituents of political cognition: Race, party politics, and the alliance detection system" CognitionVol 140 24 39 [Crossref]
76. Pratto F., John O. P. 2005"Automatic Vigilance: The Aention-Grabbing Power of negative Social Information" Social cognition: key readingsVol 250
77. Purzycki B. G., Willard A. K. 2016"mci theory: a critical discussion" Religion, Brain & BehaviorVol 6 3 207 248 [Crossref]
78. Rozin P., Royzman E. B. 2001"Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion" Personality and social psychology reviewVol 5 4 296 320 [Crossref]
79. Schram R. 2010"Witches’ wealth: witchcraft, confession, and Christianity in Auhelawa, Papua New Guinea" Journal of the Royal Anthropological InstituteVol 16 4 726 742 [Crossref]
80. Skowronski J. J., Carlston D. E. 1989"Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanations" Psychological bulletinVol 105 1 131 [Crossref]
81. Soderstrom N. C., McCabe D. P. 2011"Are survival processing memory advantages based on ancestral priorities?" Psychonomic Bulletin & ReviewVol 18 3 564 569 [Crossref]
82. Sørensen J. 2007"Acts that work: A cognitive approach to ritual agency" Method & Theory in the Study of ReligionVol 19 3 281 300 [Crossref]
83. Stadler J. J. 2003"The young, the rich, and the beautiful: Secrecy, suspicion and discourses of aids in the South African lowveld" African journal of AIDS researchVol 2 2 127 139 [Crossref]
84. Stephan W. G., Ybarra O., Martnez C. M., Schwarzwald J., Tur-Kaspa M. 1998"Prejudice toward immigrants to Spain and Israel: An integrated threat theory analysis" Journal of Cross-Cultural PsychologyVol 29 4 559 576 [Crossref]
85. Tajfel H., Billig M. G., Bundy R. P., Flament C. 1971"Social categorization and intergroup behaviour" European Journal of Social PsychologyVol 1 2 149 178 [Crossref]
86. Tenkorang E. Y., Gyimah S. O., Maticka-Tyndale E., Adjei J. 2011"Superstition, witchcraft and hiv prevention in sub-Saharan Africa: The case of Ghana" Culture, health & sexualityVol 13 9 1001 1014 [Crossref]
87. van Elk M. 2013"Paranormal believers are more prone to illusory agency detection than skeptics" Consciousness and cognitionVol 22 3 1041 1046 [Crossref]
88. van Elk M. 2015"Perceptual biases in relation to paranormal and conspiracy beliefs" PloS oneVol 10 6 e0130422 [Crossref]
89. van Elk M., Rutjens B. T., van der Pligt J., Van Harreveld F. 2016"Priming of supernatural agent concepts and agency detection" Religion, Brain & BehaviorVol 6 1 4 33 [Crossref]
90. van Leeuwen F., Parren N., Miton H., Boyer P. 2016"Individual Choose-to-Transmit Decisions Reveal Little Preference for Transmitting Negative or High-Arousal Content" Journal of cognition and culture
91. Vasquez E. A., Wenborne L., Peers M., Alleyne E., Ellis K. 2015"Any of them will do: In‐group identification, out‐group entitativity, and gang membership as predictors of group‐based retribution" Aggressive behaviorVol 41 3 242 252 [Crossref]
92. Walker C. J., Blaine B. 1991"The virulence of dread rumors: A field experiment" Language & CommunicationVol 11 4 291 297 [Crossref]
93. Waytz A., Morewedge C. K., Epley N., Monteleone G., Gao J.-H., Cacioppo J. T. 2010"Making sense by making sentient: effectance motivation increases anthropomorphism" Journal of personality and social psychologyVol 99 3 410 [Crossref]
94. Weinstein Y., Bugg J. M., Roediger H. L. 2008"Can the survival recall advantage be explained by basic memory processes?" Memory & CognitionVol 36 5 913 919 [Crossref]
95. Williams K. D., Cheung C. K., Choi W. 2000"Cyberostracism: effects of being ignored over the Internet" Journal of personality and social psychologyVol 79 5 748 [Crossref]
96. Yamagishi T., Jin N., Kiyonari T. 1999"Bounded generalized reciprocity" Adv Group ProcessVol 16 161 197
97. Yamagishi T., Mifune N. 2008"Does shared group membership promote altruism? Fear, greed, and reputation" Rationality and SocietyVol 20 1 5 30 [Crossref]
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/15685373-12340015
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/15685373-12340015
2017-11-22
2018-06-20

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Journal of Cognition and Culture — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation