Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

THE PROBLEM WITH COURTING A CYLINDRICAL OBJECT: HOW DOES AN AMOROUS MALE SNAKE DETERMINE WHICH END IS WHICH?

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

[In order to achieve copulation, a male snake must align his body in the same direction as his mate. This may not be a trivial problem in an elongate limbless animal, especially when the male is much smaller than the female (and hence, his head is far from hers) and when she is simultaneously being courted by many other males (so that much of her body is not visible). This is exactly the situation experienced by red-sided gartersnakes (Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis) at communal dens in Manitoba. To clarify this question, we modified cues available to males by manipulating various attributes of females (direction of movement, presence of head and tail, concentration of sex pheromones, etc.). Male alignment during courtship does not depend on the visual stimulus provided by the female′s head or tail. Instead, male alignment is influenced by the female′s direction of movement, and by pheromonal stimuli from her skin. We suggest that courting male snakes obtain directional information via their characteristic 'chin-pressing′ behaviour. By pressing firmly on each scale with his chin as he passes over it, a male moving anteriorly (but not posteriorly) along the female′s body will open a small gap between that scale and the next most anterior one. This gap enables his tongue-tips to penetrate more easily towards the scale-hinge area (overlain by the more anterior scale) where sex pheromones are concentrated. Thus, the direction of scale overlap provides pheromonal as well as physical cues that permit males to align correctly for copulation., In order to achieve copulation, a male snake must align his body in the same direction as his mate. This may not be a trivial problem in an elongate limbless animal, especially when the male is much smaller than the female (and hence, his head is far from hers) and when she is simultaneously being courted by many other males (so that much of her body is not visible). This is exactly the situation experienced by red-sided gartersnakes (Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis) at communal dens in Manitoba. To clarify this question, we modified cues available to males by manipulating various attributes of females (direction of movement, presence of head and tail, concentration of sex pheromones, etc.). Male alignment during courtship does not depend on the visual stimulus provided by the female′s head or tail. Instead, male alignment is influenced by the female′s direction of movement, and by pheromonal stimuli from her skin. We suggest that courting male snakes obtain directional information via their characteristic 'chin-pressing′ behaviour. By pressing firmly on each scale with his chin as he passes over it, a male moving anteriorly (but not posteriorly) along the female′s body will open a small gap between that scale and the next most anterior one. This gap enables his tongue-tips to penetrate more easily towards the scale-hinge area (overlain by the more anterior scale) where sex pheromones are concentrated. Thus, the direction of scale overlap provides pheromonal as well as physical cues that permit males to align correctly for copulation.]

Affiliations: 1: School of Biological Sciences A08, University of Sydney, NSW 2006 Australia; 2: Zoology Department, Oregon State University, Cordley Hall 3029, Corvallis, Oregon 97331-2914, USA

10.1163/156853900502312
/content/journals/10.1163/156853900502312
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
6
3
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/156853900502312
Loading

Data & Media loading...

http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/156853900502312
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/156853900502312
2000-06-01
2016-12-08

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation