Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Compulsory Regime and Control of Environment in Animal Behaviour Iii. Light Level Preferences of Small Noctural Mammals

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

[Twenty individuals belonging to the rodent species Glaucomys volans, Microtus californicus, M. pennsylvanicus, Neotoma lepida, Peromyscus californicus, P. crinitus, P. floridanus, and Thomomys bottae, and the didelphid species Marmosa mitis were studied in activity-wheel enclosures in which they could control ambient illumination in 10 steps (darkness to 200 lux) by instrumental means. At each illuminance level we determined the time spent running (active time) and not running (inactive time), running speed, number of visits, and visit lengths. During activity, most individuals were highly selective for illuminance. Behaviour not only was generally species specific, but conspecifics often gave identical results. Since the illuminance preferences generally correlated well with habits and habitat preferences in the wild, volitional control of illumination tests probably give a reliable indication of the overall state of adaptation of the visual system of small nocturnal mammals; such tests could serve as Independent guides for Interpreting retinal histology. Both the mouse opossums and gopher spent most of their active time in total darkness and much of the remaining time at level 2 (0.7% of starlight). For the flying squirrels, active time generally peaked at level 2 or 3 (2.1 or 7.5 × 10-5 lux), for the Florida mice it peaked at level 5 (50% of the light on a clear moonless night), and for the California mice at level 7 (about 3 times full moon). lndividual woodrats and canyon mice had the most spread between the positions of their peaks - levels 5 to 7 for the former and 5 to 8 for the latter. Meadow voles were the only animals to have dual illuminance preferences (darkness and level 4). These dual preferences may be related to the fact that meadow voles need to feed at regular intervals and are active around-the-clock in the wild. Components of persisting physiological cycles, particularly digestion, may have influenced the degree to which visual and non-visual factors determined to voles' illuminance preferences; thus, non-visual factors may have predominated during primarily digestive phases and visual ones during primarily non-digestive ones. Eight of the animals showed preference shifts to dimmer levels during inactivity; 3 of these 8 showed marked shifts to darkness. For 13 of the 20 animals, running was fastest in the illuminance range preferred for activity. The results for the gopher and flying squirrels accord with our hypothesis that the proportionality of the running speed of certain rodents to illuminance is the result of selection for individuals that run as fast as navigational safety permits during twilights. Thus, factors that lead to such selection would not be expected to play a significant role for gophers and flying squirrels, and these two species did not exhibit the speed-illuminance dependence., Twenty individuals belonging to the rodent species Glaucomys volans, Microtus californicus, M. pennsylvanicus, Neotoma lepida, Peromyscus californicus, P. crinitus, P. floridanus, and Thomomys bottae, and the didelphid species Marmosa mitis were studied in activity-wheel enclosures in which they could control ambient illumination in 10 steps (darkness to 200 lux) by instrumental means. At each illuminance level we determined the time spent running (active time) and not running (inactive time), running speed, number of visits, and visit lengths. During activity, most individuals were highly selective for illuminance. Behaviour not only was generally species specific, but conspecifics often gave identical results. Since the illuminance preferences generally correlated well with habits and habitat preferences in the wild, volitional control of illumination tests probably give a reliable indication of the overall state of adaptation of the visual system of small nocturnal mammals; such tests could serve as Independent guides for Interpreting retinal histology. Both the mouse opossums and gopher spent most of their active time in total darkness and much of the remaining time at level 2 (0.7% of starlight). For the flying squirrels, active time generally peaked at level 2 or 3 (2.1 or 7.5 × 10-5 lux), for the Florida mice it peaked at level 5 (50% of the light on a clear moonless night), and for the California mice at level 7 (about 3 times full moon). lndividual woodrats and canyon mice had the most spread between the positions of their peaks - levels 5 to 7 for the former and 5 to 8 for the latter. Meadow voles were the only animals to have dual illuminance preferences (darkness and level 4). These dual preferences may be related to the fact that meadow voles need to feed at regular intervals and are active around-the-clock in the wild. Components of persisting physiological cycles, particularly digestion, may have influenced the degree to which visual and non-visual factors determined to voles' illuminance preferences; thus, non-visual factors may have predominated during primarily digestive phases and visual ones during primarily non-digestive ones. Eight of the animals showed preference shifts to dimmer levels during inactivity; 3 of these 8 showed marked shifts to darkness. For 13 of the 20 animals, running was fastest in the illuminance range preferred for activity. The results for the gopher and flying squirrels accord with our hypothesis that the proportionality of the running speed of certain rodents to illuminance is the result of selection for individuals that run as fast as navigational safety permits during twilights. Thus, factors that lead to such selection would not be expected to play a significant role for gophers and flying squirrels, and these two species did not exhibit the speed-illuminance dependence.]

Affiliations: 1: (University of California, Los Angeles, Calif., U.S.A

10.1163/156853976X00361
/content/journals/10.1163/156853976x00361
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
6
3
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/156853976x00361
Loading

Data & Media loading...

http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/156853976x00361
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/156853976x00361
1976-01-01
2016-12-10

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation