Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

The frequency of vocal mimicry associated with danger varies due to proximity to nest and nesting stage in a passerine bird

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

Several species of birds vocally imitate sounds associated with danger. Two anecdotal studies suggest that such ‘danger mimicry’ increases during nesting, but such a relationship has not been quantitatively demonstrated. Sri Lanka drongos (Dicrurus paradiseus lophorhinus) are known to imitate predators and other species’ mobbing and alarm calls in alarm contexts. Here we investigated whether drongos vary their production of danger mimicry in different nesting stages (building, incubation, nests with hatchlings, fledglings still outside of mixed-species flocks), and when foraging away from young in mixed-species flocks. We recorded drongos over two breeding seasons at 14 different nesting trees, used year-after-year. We found that of all the types of danger mimicry, imitation of predators was the most common and exclusive to drongos that had young offspring. Such predator mimicry was observed at a higher rate during the hatchling and fledgling stages compared to incubation or flocks. Danger mimicry did not, however, increase during this stage in isolation: drongo species-specific alarm calls also increased, and the close connection between these two types of calls did not appear to change. Although it is possible that the association between danger mimicry and species-specific alarm calls could help young birds learn sounds associated with danger, the performance of this behaviour does not seem exclusive enough to interactions between adult drongos and their offspring to meet functional definitions of teaching.

Affiliations: 1: aXishuangbanna Tropical Botanical Garden, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Menglun, Mengla, Yunnan 666303, P.R. China; 2: bField Ornithology Group of Sri Lanka, Department of Zoology, University of Colombo, Colombo, Sri Lanka

10.1163/1568539X-00003123
/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003123
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
6
3
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003123
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Bates D. , Maechler M. , Bolker B. ( 2011). Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. — Available online at: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html( accessed December 2012).
2. Bretz F. , Hothorn T. , Westfall T. ( 2010). Multiple comparisons using R. — CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL. http://dx.doi.org/10.1201/9781420010909
3. Caro T.M. , Hauser M.D. ( 1992). "Is there teaching in nonhuman animals?"— Q. Rev. Biol. Vol 67: 151- 174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/417553
4. Chu M. ( 2001). "Heterospecific responses to scream calls and vocal mimicry by Phainopeplas ( Phainopepla nitens) in distress". — Behaviour Vol 138: 775- 787. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853901752233406
5. Coleman S.W. , Patricelli G.L. , Coyle B. , Siani J. , Borgia G. ( 2007). "Female preferences drive the evolution of mimetic accuracy in male sexual displays". — Biol. Lett. Vol 3: 463- 466. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2007.0234
6. Firth D. ( 1993). "Bias reduction of maximum likelihood estimates". — Biometrika Vol 80: 27- 38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/biomet/80.1.27
7. Flower T. ( 2011). "Fork-tailed drongos use deceptive mimicked alarm calls to steal food". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 278: 1548- 1555. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1932
8. Goodale E. , Kotagama S.W. ( 2006). "Context-dependent mimicry by a passerine bird". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 273: 875- 880. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2005.3392
9. Haff T.M. , Magrath R.D. ( 2012). "Learning to listen? Nestling response to heterospecific alarm calls". — Anim. Behav. Vol 84: 1401- 1410. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.005
10. Igic B. , Magrath R.D. ( 2013). "Fidelity of vocal mimicry: identification and accuracy of mimicry of heterospecific alarm calls by the brown thornbill". — Anim. Behav. Vol 85: 593- 603. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.022
11. Kelley L.A. , Healy S.D. ( 2011). "The mimetic repertoire of the spotted bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus maculatus ". — Naturwissenschaften Vol 98: 501- 507. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00114-011-0794-z
12. Kelley L.A. , Healy S.D. ( 2012). "Vocal mimicry in spotted bowerbirds is associated with an alarming context". — J. Avian Biol. Vol 43: 1- 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-048X.2012.05863.x
13. Li J. , Lin S. , Wang Y. , Zhang Z. ( 2009). "Nest-dismantling behavior of the hair-crested drongo in central China: an adaptive behavior for increasing fitness?"— Condor Vol 111: 197- 201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/cond.2009.080051
14. Morton E.S. ( 1976). "Vocal mimicry in the thick-billed Euphonia". — Wilson Bull. Vol 88: 485- 487.
15. Oatley T.B. ( 1969). "The functions of vocal imitation by African Cossyphas". — Ostrich Vol 40: 85- 89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00306525.1969.9639112
16. Rasmussen P.C. , Anderton J.C. ( 2005). Birds of South Asia: the Ripley guide. — Lynx, Barcelona.
17. Ratnayake C.P. , Goodale E. , Kotagama S.W. ( 2010). "Two sympatric species of passerine birds imitate the same raptor calls in alarm contexts". — Naturwissenschaften Vol 97: 103- 108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00114-009-0617-7
18. Satischandra S.H.K. , Kudavidanage E.P. , Kotagama S.W. , Goodale E. ( 2007). "The benefits of joining mixed-species flocks for a sentinel nuclear species, the Greater Racket-tailed Drongo Dicrurus paradiseus ". — Forktail Vol 23: 145- 148.
19. Thornton A. , Raihani N.J. ( 2010). "Identifying teaching in wild animals". — Learn. Behav. Vol 38: 297- 309. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/LB.38.3.297
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003123
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003123
2014-01-01
2016-12-08

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation