Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Song stability and neighbour recognition in a migratory songbird, the black redstart

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

Territorial male songbirds have the ability to discriminate between the songs of their neighbours and those of strangers and for a few species it has been shown that they maintain this ability from one breeding season to the next. To better understand the acoustic basis of this long-term discrimination ability we studied song stability across two breeding seasons in a migratory songbird with high inter-annual return rates and territory stability, the black redstart, Phoenicurus ochruros. Strophe repertoires of 14 males (≥2 years old) were stable from one breeding season to the next and high strophe sharing occurred for males within the same group of houses or hamlets (81%) in contrast to only limited sharing between different hamlets (15%). However, subtle differences exist between the renditions of the same strophe sung by neighbouring males and these differences equally show an inter-annual stability, providing an acoustic basis for long-term discrimination abilities. Playback tests showed the existence of a strong dear-enemy effect: males reacted less aggressively to the familiar, often shared song of a neighbour than to a stranger unshared song and this pattern was maintained when birds returned from migration one year later. We discuss on one side the possible mechanisms leading to the observed patterns of song sharing and on the other side the significance of stable vocal signatures for neighbour recognition.

Affiliations: 1: aUniversité de Saint-Etienne, Equipe ‘Neuro-Ethologie Sensorielle’, CNPS, CNRS UMR 8195, Saint-Etienne, France; 2: bLaboratoire d’Ethologie et Cognition Comparées, 200 Avenue de la République, BSL, Université Paris Ouest Nanterre La Défense, 92001 Nanterre, France

10.1163/1568539X-00003129
/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003129
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
6
3
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003129
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Apfelbeck B. , Kiefer S. , Mortega G.M. , Goymann W. , Kipper S. ( 2012). "Testosterone affects song modulation during simulated territorial intrusions in male black redstarts ( Phoenicurus ochruros)". — PLoS ONE Vol 7: e52009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/annotation/89b74b56-3f48-4ee2-9da1-699b3fdea564
2. Beecher M.D. , Akçay C. (in press). "Friends and enemies: how social dynamics shape communication and song learning in song sparrows". — In: Encyclopedia of animal behavior, Vol Vol. 3( Yasukawa K. , ed.). Praeger, Westport, CT.
3. Beecher M.D. , Brenowitz E.A. ( 2005). "Functional aspects of song learning in songbirds". — Trends Ecol. Evol. Vol 20: 143- 149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.004
4. Blumenrath S.H. , Dabelsteen T. , Pedersen S.B. ( 2007). "Vocal neighbour–mate discrimination in female great tits despite high song similarity". — Anim. Behav. Vol 73: 789- 796. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.07.011
5. Briefer E. , Aubin T. , Lehongre K. , Rybak F. ( 2008). "How to identify dear enemies: the group signature in the complex song of the skylark Alauda arvensis ". — J. Exp. Biol. Vol 211: 317- 326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.013359
6. Cucco M. , Malacarne G. ( 1999). "Is the song of black redstart males an honest signal of status?"— Condor Vol 101: 689- 694. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1370203
7. Davis M.S. ( 1987). "Acoustically mediated neighbor recognition in the North American bullfrog, Rana catesbeiana ". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 21: 185- 190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00303209
8. Draganoiu T.I. , Nagle L. , Musseau R. , Kreutzer M. ( 2005). "Parental care and brood division in a songbird, the black redstart". — Behaviour Vol 142: 1495- 1514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853905774831828
9. Draganoiu T.I. , Nagle L. , Musseau R. , Kreutzer M. ( 2006a). "In a songbird, the black redstart, parents use acoustic cues to discriminate between their different fledglings". — Anim. Behav. Vol 71: 1039- 1046. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.06.022
10. Draganoiu T.I. , Pasteau M. , Nagle L. ( 2006b). "Do different elements of black redstart song have different threat values?"— J. Ornithol. Vol 147: 158- 159.
11. Fisher J. ( 1954). "Evolution and bird sociality". — In: Evolution as a process( Huxley J. , Hardy A.C. , Ford E.B. , eds). Allen and Unwin, London, p.  71- 83.
12. Forstmeier W. , Balsby T.J.S. ( 2002). "Why mated dusky warblers sing so much: territory guarding and male quality announcement". — Behaviour Vol 139: 89- 111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685390252902300
13. Fox S.F. , Baird T.A. ( 1992). "The dear enemy phenomenon in the collared lizard, Crotaphytus collaris, with a cautionary note on experimental methodology". — Anim. Behav. Vol 44: 780- 782. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80306-9
14. Gilbert G. , Tyler G.A. , Smith K.W. ( 2002). "Local annual survival of booming male great bittern Botaurus stellarisin Britain, in the period 1990–1999". — Ibis Vol 144: 51- 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.0019-1019.2001.00012.x
15. Godard R. ( 1991). "Long-term memory of individual neighbours in a migratory songbird". — Nature Vol 350: 228- 229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/350228a0
16. Grava T. , Mathevon N. , Place E. , Balluet P. ( 2008). "Individual acoustic monitoring of the European eagle owl Bubo bubo ". — Ibis Vol 150: 279- 287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919X.2007.00776.x
17. Hardouin L.A. , Tabel P. , Bretagnolle V. ( 2006). "Neighbour–stranger discrimination in the little owl, Athene noctua ". — Anim. Behav. Vol 72: 105- 112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.09.020
18. Jaeger R.G. ( 1981). "Dear enemy recognition and the costs of agression between salamanders". — Am. Nat. Vol 117: 962- 974. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/283780
19. Kipper S. , Kiefer S. ( 2010). "Age-related changes in birds’ singing styles: on fresh tunes and fading voices?"— Adv. Stud. Behav. Vol 41: 77- 118.
20. Landmann A. , Kollinsky C. ( 1995). "Territory defence in black redstarts, Phoenicurus ochruros: effects of intruder and owner age?"— Ethology Vol 101: 121- 129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1995.tb00351.x
21. Langen T.A. , Tripet F. , Nonacs P. ( 2000). "The red and the black: habituation and the dear-enemy phenomenon in two desert Pheidoleants". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 48: 285- 292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002650000223
22. Mathevon N. , Aubin T. , Vielliard J. , Da Silva M.L. , Sebe F. , Boscolo D. ( 2008). "Singing in the rain forest: how a tropical bird song transfers information". — PLoS ONE Vol 3: e1580. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001580
23. McGregor P.K. , Krebs J.R. ( 1982). "Song types in a population of great tits ( Parus major): their distribution, abundance and acquisition by individuals". — Behaviour Vol 79: 126- 152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853982X00210
24. Musseau R. ( 2007). Stratégies d’occupation spatiale chez le rougequeue noir ( Phoenicurus ochruros) en période de reproduction. Habitat, fidélité, philopatrie et dispersion. — Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Paris.
25. Myrberg A.A. , Riggio R.J. ( 1985). "Acoustically mediated recognition by a coral reef fish ( Pomacentrus partitus)". — Anim. Behav. Vol 33: 411- 416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80065-8
26. Nelson D.A. , Poesel A. ( 2009). "Does learning produce song conformity or novelty in white-crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys?"— Anim. Behav. Vol 78: 433- 440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.04.028
27. Nicolai B. ( 1992). "Gesangdialekt beim Hausrotschwanz ( Phoenicurus ochruros): Tradierung und Gesangslernen". — Rudolstädter Nat. Hist. Schr. Vol 4: 83- 90.
28. Nordby J.J. , Campbell S.E. , Beecher M.D. ( 2007). "Selective attrition and individual song repertoire development in song sparrows". — Anim. Behav. Vol 74: 1413- 1418. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.02.008
29. Osiejuk T.S. , Ratyńska K. , Cygan J.P. , Dale S. ( 2005). "Frequency shift in homologue syllables of the ortolan bunting Emberiza hortulana ". — Behav. Proc. Vol 68: 69- 83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2004.11.005
30. Peake T.M. , Mcgregor P.K. , Smith K.W. , Tyler G. , Gilbert G. , Green R.E. ( 1998). "Individuality in corncrake Crex crexvocalizations". — Ibis Vol 140: 120- 127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919X.1998.tb04548.x
31. Rosell F. , Bjørkøyli T. ( 2002). "A test of the dear enemy phenomenon in the Eurasian beaver". — Anim. Behav. Vol 66: 1059- 1067.
32. Sedlácek O. , Cikánová B. , Fuchs R. ( 2006). "Heterospecific rival recognition in the black redstart ( Phoenicurus ochruros)". — Ornis Fennica Vol 83: 153- 161.
33. Stoddard P.K. ( 1996). "Vocal recognition of neighbours by territorial passerines". — In: Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds( Kroodsma D.E. , Miller E.H. , eds). Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, p.  356- 374.
34. Temeles E.J. ( 1994). "The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they ‘dear enemies’?"— Anim. Behav. Vol 47: 339- 350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1047
35. Weeden J.S. , Falls J.B. ( 1959). "Differential responses of male ovenbirds to recorded songs of neighboring and more distant individuals". — Auk Vol 76: 343- 351. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/4081781
36. Weggler M. ( 2001). "Age-related reproductive success in dichromatic male black redstarts Phoenicurus ochruros: why are yearlings handicapped?"— Ibis Vol 143: 264- 272. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919X.2001.tb04482.x
37. Węgrzyn E. , Lniowki K. , Osiejuk T.S. ( 2009). "Introduce yourself at the beginning–possible identification function of the initial part of the song in the great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus ". — Ornis Fennica Vol 86: 61- 70.
38. Wilson E.O. ( 1975). Sociobiology, the abridged edition. — Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
39. Wilson P.L. , Vehrencamp S.L. ( 2001). "A test of the deceptive mimicry hypothesis in song-sharing song sparrows". — Anim. Behav. Vol 62: 1197- 1205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1863
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003129
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003129
2014-01-01
2016-12-02

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation