Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Context influences spatial frames of reference in bonobos (Pan paniscus)

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

Primates must solve complex spatial problems when foraging, such as finding patchy resources and navigating between different locations. However, the nature of the cognitive representations supporting these types of behaviors is currently unclear. In humans, there has been great debate concerning the relative importance of egocentric representations (which are viewer-dependent) versus allocentric representations (which are based on aspects of the external environment). Comparative studies of nonhuman apes can illuminate which aspects of human spatial cognition are shared with other primates, versus which aspects are unique to our lineage. The current studies therefore examined spatial cognitive development in one of our closest living relatives, bonobos (Pan paniscus) across contexts. The first study assessed how younger bonobos encode locations in a place-response task in which apes first learn that one of two locations is consistently baited with a reward, and then must approach the two locations from a flipped perspective. The second study examined how a larger age sample of bonobos responded to a spatial relations task in which they first experience that one location is baited, and then can generalize this learning to a new set of targets. Results indicated that while bonobos exhibited a predominantly allocentric strategy in the first study, they consistently exhibited an egocentric strategy in the second. Together, these results show that bonobos can use both strategies to encode spatial information, and illuminate the complementary contributions to cognition made by egocentric and allocentric representations.

Affiliations: 1: Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA

10.1163/1568539X-00003189
/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003189
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
6
3
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003189
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Acredolo L.P. ( 1978). "Development of spatial orientation in infancy". — Dev. Psychol. Vol 14: 224- 234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.14.3.224
2. Acredolo L.P. ( 1979). "Laboratory versus home: the effect of environment on the 9-month-old infant’s choice of spatial reference system". — Dev. Psychol. Vol 15: 666- 667. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.15.6.666
3. Acredolo L.P. , Evans D. ( 1980). "Developmental changes in the effects of landmarks on infant spatial behavior". — Dev. Psychol. Vol 16: 312- 318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.16.4.312
4. Albiach-Serrano A. , Call J. , Barth J. ( 2010). "Great apes track hidden objects after changes in the objects’ position and in subject’s orientation". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 72: 349- 359.
5. Baayen R.H. ( 2008). Analyzing linguistic data. A practical introduction to statistics. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511801686
6. Bates D. ( 2010). The LME4 package: linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. — Available online at .
7. Bolker B.M. , Brooks M.E. , Clark C.J. , Geange S.W. , Poulsen J.R. , Stevens M.H.H. , White J.S.S. ( 2008). "Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution". — Trends Ecol. Evol. Vol 24: 127- 135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2008.10.008
8. Brown P. , Levinson S. ( 1992). "‘Left’ and ‘right’ in Tenejapa: investigating a linguistic conceptual gap". — Z. Phonetik Sprachwissensch. Kommunikationsforsch. Vol 45: 590- 611.
9. Brown P. , Levinson S.C. ( 2000). "Frames of spatial reference and their acquisition in Tenejapan Tzeltal". — In: Culture, thought, and development( Nucci L. , Saxe G. , Turiel E. , eds). Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, p.  167- 197.
10. Burgess N. ( 2006). "Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine". — Trends Cogn. Sci. Vol 10: 551- 557. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.005
11. Burgess N. ( 2008). "Spatial cognition and the brain". — Ann. NY Acad. Sci. Vol 1124: 77- 97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.002
12. Cheng K. ( 1986). "A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation". — Cognition Vol 23: 149- 178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90041-7
13. Gallistel C.R. ( 1990). The organization of learning. — Bradford Books/MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
14. Gentner D. ( 2007). "Spatial cognition in apes and humans". — Trends Cogn. Sci. Vol 11: 192- 194. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.03.002
15. Hare B. ( 2007). "From nonhuman to human mind: what changed and why". — Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. Vol 16: 60- 64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00476.x
16. Hare B. , Wobber V. , Wrangam R. ( 2012). "The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression". — Anim. Behav. Vol 83: 573- 585. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.007
17. Haun D.B.M. , Rapold C.J. , Call J. , Janzen G. , Levinson S.C. ( 2006a). "Cognitive cladistics and cultural override in Hominid spatial cognition". — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol 103: 17568- 17573. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0607999103
18. Haun D.B.M. , Call J. , Janzen G. , Levinson S.C. ( 2006b). "Evolutionary psychology of spatial representations in the Hominidae". — Curr. Biol. Vol 16: 1736- 1740. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.049
19. Hermer L. , Spelke E. ( 1994). "A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children". — Nature Vol 370: 57- 59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/370057a0
20. Hermer-Vazquez L. , Spelke E. , Katnelson A.S. ( 1999). "Sources of flexibility in human cognition: dual-task studies of space and language". — Cogn. Psychol. Vol 39: 3- 36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0713
21. Hermer-Vazquez L. , Moffet A. , Munkholm P. ( 2001). "Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks". — Cognition Vol 79: 263- 299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00120-7
22. Herrmann E. , Hare B. , Call J. , Tomasello M. ( 2010). "Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees". — PLoS One Vol 5: e12438.
23. Hoffman M.L. , Beran M.J. ( 2006). "Chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes) remember the location of a hidden food item after altering their orientation to a spatial array". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 120: 389- 393. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.120.4.389
24. Hribar A. , Call J. ( 2011). "Great apes use landmark cues over spatial relations to find hidden food". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 14: 623- 635. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0397-2
25. Hribar A. , Haun D. , Call J. ( 2011). "Great apes’ strategies to map spatial relations". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 14: 511- 523. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0385-6
26. Janson C.H. ( 1998). "Experimental evidence for spatial memory in wild brown capuchin monkeys ( Cebus apella)". — Anim. Behav. Vol 55: 1229- 1243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0688
27. Janson C.H. ( 2007). "Experimental evidence for route integration and strategic planning in wild capuchin monkeys". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 10: 341- 356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0079-2
28. Janson C.H. , Byrne R. ( 2007). "What wild primates know about resources: opening up the black box". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 10: 357- 367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0080-9
29. Kanngiesser P. , Call J. ( 2010). "Bonobos, chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans use feature and spatial cues in two spatial memory tasks". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 13: 419- 430. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0291-3
30. Kano T. ( 1992). The last ape: pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology. — Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA.
31. Learmonth A.E. , Newcombe N.S. , Sheridan N. , Jones M. ( 2008). "Why size counts: children’s spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures". — Dev. Sci. Vol 11: 414- 426. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00686.x
32. Lee S.A. , Spelke E. ( 2010). "Two systems of spatial representation underlying navigation". — Exp. Brain Res. Vol 206: 179- 188. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2349-5
33. Levinson S.C. ( 1996). "Frames of reference and Molyneux’s question: cross-linguistic evidence". — In: Language and space. Language, speech, and communication( Bloom P. , Peterson M.A. , Nadel L. , Garrett M.F. , eds). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, p.  385- 436.
34. Levinson S.C. , Kita S. , Haun D.B.M. , Rasch B.H. ( 2002). "Returning the tables: language affects spatial reasoning". — Cognition Vol 84: 144- 188. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00045-8
35. Li P. , Gleitman L. ( 2002). "Turning the tables: language and spatial reasoning". — Cognition Vol 83: 265- 294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00009-4
36. Maguire E.A. , Burgess N. , Donnett J.G. , Frackowiak R.S.J. , Frith C.D. , O’Keefe J. ( 1998). "Knowning where and getting there: a human navigation network". — Science Vol 280: 921- 923. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.280.5365.921
37. Majid A. , Bowerman M. , Kita S. , Haun D. , Levinson S. ( 2004). "Can language restructure cognition? The case for space". — Trends Cogn. Sci. Vol 8: 108- 114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.003
38. Malenky R.K. , Wrangham R.W. ( 1993). "A quantitative comparison of terrestrial herbaceous food consumption by Pan paniscusin the Lomako Forest, Zaire, and Pan troglodytesin the Kibale Forest, Uganda". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 32: 1- 12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350320102
39. Martin-Ordas G. , Haun D. , Colmenares F. , Call J. ( 2010). "Keeping track of time: evidence for episodic-like memory in great apes". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 13: 331- 340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0282-4
40. Matsuzawa T. ( 2007). "Comparative cognitive development". — Dev. Sci. Vol 10: 97- 103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00570.x
41. Matsuzawa T. , Tomonaga M. , Tanaka M. (eds) ( 2006). Cognitive development in chimpanzees. — Springer, Tokyo.
42. Mendes N. ( 2008). "Spatial memory in chimpanzees: single-trial learning and 24 hour and 3 month long-term memory". — Folia Primatol. Vol 79: 283- 304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000134266
43. Menzel C.R. , Savage-Rumbaugh E.S. , Menzel E.W. ( 2002). "Bonobo ( Pan paniscus) spatial memory and communication in a 20-hectare forest". — Int. J. Primatol. Vol 23: 601- 619. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1014925802015
44. Menzel E.W. ( 1973). "Chimpanzee spatial memory organization". — Science Vol 182: 943- 945. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.182.4115.943
45. Newcombe N.S. , Huttenlocher J. ( 2006). "Development of spatial cognition". — In: Handbook of child psychology, Vol Vol. II: cognition, perception, and language( Damon W. , Lerner R.M. , Kuhn D. , Siegler R.S. , eds). Wiley, New York, NY, p.  734- 776.
46. Newcombe N.S. , Ratliff K.R. ( 2007). "Explaining the development of spatial reorientation: modularity-plus-language versus the emergence of adaptive combination". — In: Emerging landscapes of mind: mapping the nature of change in spatial cognitive development( Plumert J. , Spencer J. , eds). Oxford University Press, New York, NY, p.  53- 76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195189223.003.0003
47. Newcombe N.S. , Huttenlocher J. , Drummey A. , Wiley J.G. ( 1998). "The development of spatial location coding: place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years". — Cogn. Dev. Vol 13: 185- 200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90038-7
48. Normand E. , Ban S.D. , Boesch C. ( 2009). "Forest chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes verus) remember the location of numerous fruit trees". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 12: 797- 807. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0239-7
49. Normand E. , Boesch C. ( 2009). "Sophisticated Euclidean maps in forest chimpanzees". — Anim. Behav. Vol 77: 1195- 1201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.025
50. Packard M.G. ( 1996). "Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning". — Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. Vol 65: 65- 72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1996.0007
51. Packard M.G. ( 1999). "Glutamate infused posttraining into the hippocampus or caudate-putamen differentially strengthens place and response learning". — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol 96: 12881- 12886. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.96.22.12881
52. Packard M.G. ( 2009). "Exhumed from thought: basal ganglia and response learning in the plus-maze". — Behav. Brain Res. Vol 199: 24- 31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.12.013
53. Packard M.G. , Goodman J. ( 2013). "Factors that influence the relative use of multiple memory systems". — Hippocampus Vol 23: 1044- 1052. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22178
54. Pederson E. , Danziger E. , Wilkins D.G. , Levinson S.C. , Kita S. , Senft G. ( 1998). "Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization". — Language Vol 74: 557- 589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.1998.0074
55. Poldrack R.A. , Clark M.A. , Pare-Blagoev E.J. , Shohamy D. , Creso Moyan J. , Myers C. , et al , ( 2001). "Interactive memory systems in the human brain". — Nature Vol 414: 546. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/35107080
56. Poldrack R.A. , Packard M.G. ( 2003). "Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies". — Neuropsychologia Vol 41: 245- 251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00157-4
57. R Development Core Team( 2011). A language and environment for statistical computing. — .
58. Ratliff K.R. , Newcombe N.S. ( 2008). "Reorienting when clues conflct: evidence for an adaptive-combination view". — Psychol. Sci. Vol 19: 1301- 1307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02239.x
59. Rosati A.G. , Hare B. ( 2012). "Chimpanzees and bonobos exhibit divergent spatial memory development". — Dev. Sci. Vol 15: 840- 853. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01182.x
60. Rosati A.G. , Rodriguez K. , Hare B. ( 2014). "The ecology of spatial memory in four lemur species". — Anim. Cogn., in press, DOI: .
61. Rosati A.G. , Wobber V. , Hughes K. , Santos L.R. (in press). "How is human cognitive development unique?"— Comp. Dev. Psychol.
62. Sherry D.F. , Schacter D.L. ( 1987). "The evolution of multiple memory systems". — Psychol. Rev. Vol 94: 439- 454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.439
63. Shettleworth S.J. ( 1998). Cognition, evolution, and behavior. — Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
64. Spelke E. , Lee S.A. , Izard V. ( 2010). "Beyond core knowledge: natural geometry". — Cogn. Sci. Vol 1: 1- 22.
65. Tolman E.C. ( 1948). "Cognitive maps in rats and men". — Psychol. Rev. Vol 55: 189- 208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061626
66. Wang R.F. , Spelke E.S. ( 2002). "Human spatial representation: insights from animals". — Trends Cogn. Sci. Vol 6: 376- 382. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01961-7
67. White F.J. ( 1998). "Seasonality and socioecology: the importance of variation in fruit abundance to bonobo sociality". — Int. J. Primatol. Vol 19: 1013- 1027. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1020374220004
68. White F.J. , Wrangham R.W. ( 1988). "Feeding competition and patch size in the chimpanzee species Pan paniscusand Pan troglodytes ". — Behaviour Vol 105: 148- 163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853988X00494
69. White N.M. , McDonald R.J. ( 2002). "Multiple parallel memory systems in the brain of the rat". — Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. Vol 77: 125- 184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nlme.2001.4008
70. Wobber V. , Hare B. ( 2011). "Psychological health of orphan bonobos and chimpanzees in African sanctuaries". — PLoS One Vol 6: e17147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017147
71. Wobber V. , Wrangham R. , Hare B. ( 2010). "Bonobos exhibit delayed development of social behavior and cognition relative to chimpanzees". — Curr. Biol. Vol 20: 226- 230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.070
72. Wobber V. , Herrmann E. , Hare B. , Wrangham R. , Tomasello M. ( 2013). "Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes". — Dev. Psychobiol., in press, DOI: .
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003189
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003189
2015-02-10
2016-12-08

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation