Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Joint tail and vocal alarm signals of gray squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis)

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
MyBook is a cheap paperback edition of the original book and will be sold at uniform, low price.

Buy this article

Price:
$30.00+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites

image of Behaviour

Threat-specific vocalizations have been observed in primates and ground squirrels, but their contemporaneous usage with visible signals has not been experimentally analyzed for association with threat type. Here we examine the eastern gray squirrel, an arboreal squirrel that uses both vocal and tail signals as alarms. Squirrels were presented with cat and hawk models simulating natural terrestrial or aerial predator attacks and also with control objects that do not resemble predators but approach in a similar manner. Individuals responded with tail signals (twitches and flags) and vocalizations (kuks, quaas and moans), but only flags and moans are associated with predator type. Moans were elicited primarily by aerial stimuli and flags by terrestrial stimuli. Eastern gray squirrels use an alarm-signaling system in which signals in each modality potentially are associated with particular attributes of a threat or may be general alarms. Terrestrially-approaching stimuli yielded vocal and tail alarm signals regardless of whether the stimulus resembled a predator. With aerially-approaching stimuli, however, quaas were used more often when the stimulus resembled an aerial predator than when it did not. An approaching object’s physical appearance may therefore affect squirrels’ responses to aerial, but not terrestrial, objects. When the stimuli resembled real predators approaching in the natural manner (terrestrially or aerially), both tail flags and vocal moans were associated with predator type, so we also considered moans and flags together. The presence and absence of moans and flags in an alarm signaling bout yields a higher statistical index of predictive association as to whether the threat is aerial or terrestrial than does either component alone.

1. Altmann J. ( 1974). "Observational study of behavior: sampling methods". — Behaviour Vol 49: 227- 267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853974X00534
2. Barbour M.A. , Clark R.W. ( 2012). "Ground squirrel tail-flag displays alter both predatory strike and ambush site selection behaviours of rattlesnakes". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 279: 3827- 3833. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.1112
3. Barkalow F.S. , Shorten M. ( 1973). The world of the gray squirrel, 1st edn.J.B. Lippincott, New York, NY.
4. Cohen J. ( 1960). "A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales". — Educ. Psychol. Measurem. Vol 20: 37- 46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001316446002000104
5. Davis L.S. ( 1984). "Alarm calling in Richardson’s ground squirrels ( Spermophilus richardsonii)". — Z. Tierpsychol. Vol 66: 152- 164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01362.x
6. Digweed S. , Rendall D. ( 2009). "Predator-associated vocalizations in North American red squirrels ( Tamiasciurus hudsonicus): to whom are alarm calls addressed and how do they function?"— Ethology Vol 115: 1190- 1199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01709.x
7. Fitzgibbon C.D. ( 1994). "The costs and benefits of predator inspection behaviour in Thomson’s gazelles". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 34: 139- 148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00164184
8. Furrer R.D. , Manser M.B. ( 2009). "The evolution of urgency-based and functionally referential alarm calls in ground-dwelling species". — Am. Nat. Vol 173: 400- 410. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/596541
9. Hennessy D.F. , Owings D.H. , Rowe M.P. , Coss R.G. , Leger D.W. ( 1981). "The information afforded by a variable signal: constraints on snake-elicited tail flagging by California ground squirrels". — Behaviour Vol 78: 188- 226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853981X00329
10. Huynh H. , Bertolino S. , Koprowski J. , Williams G. , Thompson C. , McAlpine D. ( 2011). "The even darker side of the eastern gray squirrel ( Sciurus carolinensis): a review of global introductions, invasion biology, and pest management strategies". — Julius-Kühn-Archiv Vol 432: 31.
11. Kaufman K.B. , Rosenthal R. ( 2009). "Can you believe my eyes? The importance of interobserver reliability statistics in observations of animal behaviour". — Anim. Behav. Vol 78: 1487- 1491. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.014
12. Lariviere S. , Walton L.W. ( 1997). " Lynx rufus ". — Mamm. Spec. Vol 563: 1- 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3504533
13. Leger D.W. , Owings D.H. ( 1978). "Responses to alarm calls by California ground squirrels: effects of call structure and maternal status". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 3: 177- 186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00294989
14. Leger D.W. , Owings D.H. , Gelfand D.L. ( 1980). "Single-note vocalizations of California ground squirrels: graded signals and situation-specificity of predator and socially evoked calls". — Z. Tierpsychol. Vol 52: 227- 246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1980.tb00714.x
15. Lishak R.S. ( 1984). "Alarm vocalizations of adult gray squirrels". — J. Mammal. Vol 65: 681- 684. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1380852
16. Macedonia J.M. , Evans C.S. ( 1993). "Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals". — Ethology Vol 93: 177- 197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1993.tb00988.x
17. Manser M.B. ( 2001). "The acoustic structure of suricates’ alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 268: 2315- 2324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1773
18. Marler P. , Evans C.S. , Hauser M.D. ( 1992). "Animal signals: motivational, referential, or both?"— In: Nonverbal vocal communication: comparative and developmental approaches. Studies in Emotion and Social Interaction( Papousek H. , Jürgens U. , eds). Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, p.  66- 86.
19. McRae T.R. ( 2012). Predator-specificity of multimodal alarm signals in the eastern gray squirrel ( Sciurus carolinensis). — PhD dissertation, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL.
20. Meng H. ( 1959). "Food habits of nesting Cooper’s hawks and goshawks in New York and Pennsylvania". — Wilson Bull. Vol 71: 169- 174.
21. Otovic P. , Partan S.R. ( 2009). "Multimodal signalling in animals". — In: The new encyclopedia of neuroscience( Squire L.R. , Editor-in-Chief). Academic Press, Oxford, p.  1095- 1105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-008045046-9.01860-X
22. Owings D.H. , Leger D.W. ( 1980). "Chatter vocalizations of California ground squirrels: predator- and social-role specificity". — Z. Tierpsychol. Vol 184: 163- 184.
23. Owings D.H. , Virginia R.A. ( 1978). "Alarm calls of California ground squirrels ( Spermophilus beecheyi)". — Z. Tierpsychol. Vol 46: 58- 70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1978.tb01438.x
24. Owren M.J. , Rendall D. , Ryan M.J. ( 2010). "Redefining animal signaling: influence versus information in communication". — Biol. Philos. Vol 25: 755- 780. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10539-010-9224-4
25. Partan S.R. , Larco C.P. , Owens M.J. ( 2009). "Wild tree squirrels respond with multisensory enhancement to conspecific robot alarm behaviour". — Anim. Behav. Vol 77: 1127- 1135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.029
26. Partan S.R. , Fulmer A.G. , Gounard M.A.M. , Redmond J.E. ( 2010). "Multimodal alarm behavior in urban and rural gray squirrels studied by means of observation and a mechanical robot". — Curr. Zool. Vol 56: 313- 326.
27. Robinson S.R. ( 1981). "Alarm communication in Belding’s ground squirrels". — Z. Tierpsychol. Vol 56: 150- 168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1981.tb01293.x
28. Searcy W.A. , Nowicki S. ( 2006). "Signal interception and the use of soft song in aggressive interactions". — Ethology Vol 112: 865- 872. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01238.x
29. Seyfarth R.M. , Cheney D.L. , Marler P. ( 1980a). "Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication". — Science Vol 210: 801- 803. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7433999
30. Seyfarth R.M. , Cheney D.L. , Marler P.M. ( 1980b). "Vervet monkey alarm calls — semantic communication in a free-ranging primate". — Anim. Behav. Vol 28: 1070- 1094. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80097-2
31. Sloan J.L. , Wilson D.R. , Hare J.F. ( 2005). "Functional morphology of Richardson’s ground squirrel, Spermophilus richardsonii, alarm calls: the meaning of chirps, whistles and chucks". — Anim. Behav. Vol 70: 937- 944. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.01.013
32. Steele M.A. , Koprowski J.L. ( 2003). North American tree squirrels, 1st edn.Smithsonian Books, Washington, DC.
33. Stegmann U. ( 2013). Animal communication theory — information and influence. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139003551
34. Struhsaker T.T. ( 1967). "Auditory communication among vervet monkeys ( Cercopithecus aethiops)". — In: Social communication among primates, 1st edn.( Altmann S.A. , ed.). The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, p.  281- 324.
35. Swaisgood R.R. , Rowe M.P. , Owings D.H. ( 2003). "Antipredator responses of California ground squirrels to rattlesnakes and rattling sounds: the roles of sex, reproductive parity, and offspring age in assessment and decision-making rules". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 55: 22- 31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-003-0684-2
36. Uhl G. , Elias D.O. ( 2011). "Communication". — In: Spider behaviour: flexibility and versatility, 1st edn.( Herberstein M.E. , ed.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  127- 189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511974496.006
37. Warkentin K.J. , Keeley A.T.H. , Hare J.F. ( 2001). "Repetitive calls of juvenile Richardson’s ground squirrels ( Spermophilus richardsonii) communicate response urgency". — Can. J. Zool. Vol 79: 569- 573. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/cjz-79-4-569
38. Wheeler B.C. ( 2010). "Production and perception of situationally variable alarm calls in wild tufted capuchin monkeys ( Cebus apella nigritus)". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 64: 989- 1000. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-0914-3
39. Zuberbühler K. , Jenny D. , Bshary R. ( 1999). "The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls". — Ethology Vol 105: 477- 490. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.1999.00396.x
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003194
2014-09-02
2015-08-05

Affiliations: 1: Department of Biology, University of Miami, 215 Cox Science Center, 1301 Memorial Drive, Coral Gables, FL 33124-0421, USA

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to email alerts
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Name:*
    Email:*
    Your details
    Name:*
    Email:*
    Department:*
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
     
     
     
    Other:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library

    Thank you

    Your recommendation has been sent to your librarian.

  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation