Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Active but asocial: exploration and activity is linked to social behaviour in a colonially breeding finch

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

One largely neglected area of personality research is an individual’s propensity to move and feed alongside others. We monitored the foraging behaviour of captive zebra finches in social groups in large enclosures using a PIT-tag system, and also separately assayed activity levels in a home cage with a social partner. We found that more active individuals found novel feeders more quickly (explored faster). Furthermore, we found that more active males fed less socially than inactive males. Our findings suggest that variation in foraging behaviour in the zebra finch is driven by both personality and social factors. This work suggests that degrees of sociality — the intrinsic propensity with which individuals interact and coordinate their activity with other conspecifics — is an important component of personality, and needs to be accounted for in future work.

Affiliations: 1: Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia

10.1163/1568539X-00003272
/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003272
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
10
5
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003272
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Aplin L.M. , Farine D.R. , Mann R.P. , Sheldon B.C. ( 2014). "Individual-level personality influences social foraging and collective behaviour in wild birds". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 281: 20141016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1016
2. Aplin L.M. , Farine D.R. , Morand-Ferron J. , Cole E.F. , Cockburn A. , Sheldon B.C. ( 2013). "Individual personalities predict social behaviour in wild networks of great tits ( Parus major)". — Ecol. Lett. Vol 16: 1365- 1372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ele.12181
3. Barta Z. , Giraldeau L.-A. ( 1998). "The effect of dominance hierarchy on the use of alternative foraging tactics: a phenotype-limited producing-scrounging game". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 42: 217- 223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002650050433
4. Beauchamp G. ( 2000a). "The effect of prior residence and pair bond on scrounging choices in flocks of zebra finches, Taenopygia guttata ". — Behav. Process. Vol 52: 131- 140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(00)00132-7
5. Beauchamp G. ( 2000b). "Individual differences in activity and exploration influence leadership in pairs of foraging zebra finches". — Behaviour Vol 137: 301- 314. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853900502097
6. Beauchamp G. ( 2002). "Higher-level evolution of intraspecific flock-feeding in birds". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 51: 480- 487. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-002-0461-7
7. Beauchamp G. ( 2006). "Phenotypic correlates of scrounging behavior in zebra finches: role of foraging efficiency and dominance". — Ethology Vol 112: 873- 878. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01241.x
8. Benskin C.M.H. , Mann N. , Lachlan R. , Slater P. ( 2002). "Social learning directs feeding preferences in the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata ". — Anim. Behav. Vol 64: 823- 828. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.2005
9. Bergmüller R. , Taborsky M. ( 2010). "Animal personality due to social niche specialisation". — Trends Ecol. Evol. Vol 25: 504- 511. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2010.06.012
10. Biro P.A. , Stamps J.A. ( 2008). "Are animal personality traits linked to life-history productivity?"— Trends Ecol. Evol. Vol 23: 361- 368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2008.04.003
11. Brown C.R. ( 1988). "Enhanced foraging efficiency through information centers: a benefit of coloniality in cliff swallows". — Ecology Vol 69: 602- 613. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1941009
12. Brust V. , Wuerz Y. , Krüger O. ( 2013). "Behavioural flexibility and personality in zebra finches". — Ethology Vol 119: 559- 569. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/eth.12095
13. Budaev S.V. ( 1997). "Alternative styles in the European wrasse, Symphodus ocellatus: boldness-related schooling tendency". — Environ. Biol. Fish. Vol 49: 71- 78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1007380212475
14. Cote J. , Fogarty S. , Sih A. ( 2012). "Individual sociability and choosiness between shoal types". — Anim. Behav. Vol 83: 1469- 1476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.019
15. Dall S.R.X. , Griffith S.C. ( 2014). "An empiricist guide to animal personality variation in ecology and evolution". — Front. Ecol. Evol. Vol 2: 3. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2014.00003
16. Dardenne S. , Ducatez S. , Cote J. , Poncin P. , Stevens V. ( 2013). "Neophobia and social tolerance are related to breeding group size in a semi-colonial bird". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 67: 1317- 1327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1560-3
17. David M. , Auclair Y. , Cézilly F. ( 2011a). "Personality predicts social dominance in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, in a feeding context". — Anim. Behav. Vol 81: 219- 224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.10.008
18. David M. , Auclair Y. , Cézilly F. ( 2012). "Assessing short- and long-term repeatability and stability of personality in captive zebra finches using longitudinal data". — Ethology Vol 118: 932- 942. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2012.02085.x
19. David M. , Cézilly F. , Giraldeau L.-A. ( 2011b). "Personality affects zebra finch feeding success in a producer–scrounger game". — Anim. Behav. Vol 82: 61- 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.03.025
20. Dingemanse N.J. , de Goede P. ( 2004). "The relation between dominance and exploratory behavior is context-dependent in wild great tits". — Behav. Ecol. Vol 15: 1023- 1030. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arh115
21. Dingemanse N.J. , Both C. , Drent P.J. , van Oers K. , van Noordwijk A.J. ( 2002). "Repeatability and heritability of exploratory behaviour in great tits from the wild". — Anim. Behav. Vol 64: 929- 938. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.2006
22. Dyer J.R.G. , Croft D.P. , Morrell L.J. , Krause J. ( 2009). "Shoal composition determines foraging success in the guppy". — Behav. Ecol. Vol 20: 165- 171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn129
23. Elgar M.A. , Catterall C.P. ( 1981). "Flocking and predator surveillance in house sparrows: test of an hypothesis". — Anim. Behav. Vol 29: 868- 872. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80022-X
24. Giraldeau L.-A. , Caraco T. ( 2000). Social foraging theory. — Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
25. Goodson J.L. , Schrock S.E. , Klatt J.D. , Kabelik D. , Kingsbury M.A. ( 2009). "Mesotocin and nonapeptide receptors promote estrildid flocking behavior". — Science Vol 325: 862- 866. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1174929
26. Harcourt J.L. , Ang T.Z. , Sweetman G. , Johnstone R.A. , Manica A. ( 2009). "Social feedback and the emergence of leaders and followers". — Curr. Biol. Vol 19: 248- 252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.12.051
27. Higgins L.A. , Jones K.M. , Wayne M.L. ( 2005). "Quantitative genetics of natural variation of behavior in Drosophila melanogaster: the possible role of the social environment on creating persistent patterns of group activity". — Evolution Vol 59: 1529- 1539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2005.tb01802.x
28. IBM( 2012). IBM SPSS statistics for Windows, Version 21.0. — IBM, Armonk, NY.
29. Katz M. , Lachlan R. ( 2003). "Social learning of food types in zebra finches ( Taenopygia guttata) is directed by demonstrator sex and feeding activity". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 6: 11- 16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-003-0158-y
30. Koolhaas J.M. , Korte S.M. , de Boer S.F. , van der Vegt B.J. , van Reenen C.G. , Hopster H. , de Jong I.C. , Ruis M.A.W. , Blokhuis H.J. ( 1999). "Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology". — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. Vol 23: 925- 935. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00026-3
31. Koski S.E. ( 2011). "Social personality traits in chimpanzees: temporal stability and structure of behaviourally assessed personality traits in three captive populations". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 65: 2161- 2174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-011-1224-0
32. Kurvers R.H. , Adamczyk V.M. , van Wieren S.E. , Prins H.H. ( 2011). "The effect of boldness on decision-making in barnacle geese is group-size-dependent". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 278: 2018- 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2266
33. Kurvers R.H. , Nolet B.A. , Prins H.H. , Ydenberg R.C. , van Oers K. ( 2012). "Boldness affects foraging decisions in barnacle geese: an experimental approach". — Behav. Ecol. Vol 23: 1155- 1161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars091
34. Lindström Å. ( 1989). "Finch flock size and risk of hawk predation at a migratory stopover site". — Auk Vol 106: 225- 232.
35. Mainwaring M.C. , Beal J.L. , Hartley I.R. ( 2011). "Zebra finches are bolder in an asocial, rather than social, context". — Behav. Process. Vol 87: 171- 175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2011.03.005
36. Mariette M.M. , Griffith S.C. ( 2012). "Conspecific attraction and nest site selection in a nomadic species, the zebra finch". — Oikos Vol 121: 823- 834. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2011.20014.x
37. Mariette M.M. , Griffith S.C. ( 2013). "Does coloniality improve foraging efficiency and nestling provisioning? A field experiment in the wild zebra finch". — Ecology Vol 94: 325- 335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1890/12-0572.1
38. Mariette M.M. , Griffith S.C. ( 2014). "The adaptive significance of provisioning and foraging coordination between breeding partners". — Am. Nat. Vol 185: 270- 280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/679441
39. Mariette M.M. , Pariser E.C. , Gilby A.J. , Magrath M.J. , Pryke S.R. , Griffith S.C. ( 2011). "Using an electronic monitoring system to link offspring provisioning and foraging behavior of a wild passerine". — Auk Vol 128: 26- 35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/auk.2011.10117
40. Martins T.L.F. , Roberts M.L. , Giblin I. , Huxham R. , Evans M.R. ( 2007). "Speed of exploration and risk-taking behavior are linked to corticosterone titres in zebra finches". — Horm. Behav. Vol 52: 445- 453. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.06.007
41. McCowan L.S. , Rollins L.A. , Griffith S.C. ( 2014). "Personality in captivity: more exploratory males reproduce better in an aviary population". — Behav. Process. Vol 107: 150- 157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.08.020
42. Michelena P. , Sibbald A.M. , Erhard H.W. , McLeod J.E. ( 2009). "Effects of group size and personality on social foraging: the distribution of sheep across patches". — Behav. Ecol. Vol 20: 145- 152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn126
43. Nakagawa S. , Cuthill I.C. ( 2007). "Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: a practical guide for biologists". — Biol. Rev. Vol 82: 591- 605. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00027.x
44. Nakagawa S. , Schielzeth H. ( 2010). "Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: a practical guide for biologists". — Biol. Rev. Vol 85: 935- 956.
45. Perez E.C. , Elie J.E. , Soulage C.O. , Soula H.A. , Mathevon N. , Vignal C. ( 2012). "The acoustic expression of stress in a songbird: does corticosterone drive isolation-induced modifications of zebra finch calls?"— Horm. Behav. Vol 61: 573- 581. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.004
46. Pike T.W. , Samanta M. , Lindström J. , Royle N.J. ( 2008). "Behavioural phenotype affects social interactions in an animal network". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 275: 2515- 2520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0744
47. Pinter-Wollman N. ( 2012). "Personality in social insects: how does worker personality determine colony personality". — Curr. Zool. Vol 58: 579- 587.
48. Pruitt J.N. , Keiser C.N. ( 2014). "The personality types of key catalytic individuals shape colonies’ collective behaviour and success". — Anim. Behav. Vol 93: 87- 95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.04.017
49. R Development Core Team( 2013). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. — R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna.
50. Réale D. , Reader S.M. , Sol D. , McDougall P.T. , Dingemanse N.J. ( 2007). "Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution". — Biol. Rev. Vol 82: 291- 318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x
51. Rosa P. , Nguyen V. , Dubois F. ( 2012). "Individual differences in sampling behaviour predict social information use in zebra finches". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 66: 1259- 1265. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-012-1379-3
52. Schuett W. , Dall S.R. ( 2009). "Sex differences, social context and personality in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata ". — Anim. Behav. Vol 77: 1041- 1050. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.024
53. Schuett W. , Dall S.R. , Royle N.J. ( 2011a). "Pairs of zebra finches with similar ‘personalities’ make better parents". — Anim. Behav. Vol 81: 609- 618. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.12.006
54. Schuett W. , Godin J.G.J. , Dall S.R.X. ( 2011b). "Do female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, choose their mates based on their ‘personality’?"— Ethology Vol 117: 908- 917. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01945.x
55. Smith B. , Blumstein D. ( 2008). "Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis". — Behav. Ecol. Vol 19: 448- 455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arm144
56. Sokolowski M.B. ( 2001). "Drosophila: genetics meets behaviour". — Nature Rev. Genet. Vol 2: 879- 890. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/35098592
57. Tanner C.J. , Jackson A.L. ( 2012). "Social structure emerges via the interaction between local ecology and individual behaviour". — J. Anim. Ecol. Vol 81: 261- 267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2011.01879.x
58. Tschirren B. , Rutstein A.N. , Postma E. , Mariette M. , Griffith S.C. ( 2009). "Short-and long-term consequences of early developmental conditions: a case study on wild and domesticated zebra finches". — J. Evol. Biol. Vol 22: 387- 395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01656.x
59. van Oers K. , Klunder M. , Drent P.J. ( 2005). "Context dependence of personalities: risk-taking behavior in a social and a nonsocial situation". — Behav. Ecol. Vol 16: 716- 723. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ari045
60. Verbeek M.E.M. , Drent P.J. , Wiepkema P.R. ( 1994). "Consistent individual differences in early exploratory behaviour of male great tits". — Anim. Behav. Vol 48: 1113- 1121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1344
61. Webster M.M. , Ward A.J.W. ( 2011). "Personality and social context". — Biol. Rev. Vol 86: 759- 773. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00169.x
62. Zann R.A. ( 1996). The zebra finch: a synthesis of field and laboratory studies. — Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003272
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003272
2015-06-04
2017-11-22

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation