Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Songbirds never forget: long-lasting behavioural change triggered by a single playback event

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

Many behavioural studies rely on playback experiments. While it is known that songbirds decrease behavioural responses after short-term repeated stimulation, long-term behavioural changes due to playbacks are unknown. We studied the response to playbacks in a free-living songbird in the long-term, while also studying the repeatability of the behaviour. Locomotor behaviour (a proxy of aggressiveness) decreased one year after first exposure to a single playback. Song response, however, remained consistent, suggesting that these two behaviours may provide different information. Locomotor behaviour was less repeatable than the song response to playback, the latter showing significant between-years repeatability. To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to report long-term decrease in response to playbacks in a songbird, and that some aspects of the response to playback are repeatable. Similar studies in other species or populations of the great tit are important, to examine the generality of our findings.

Affiliations: 1: bUniversity of Antwerp, Ethology Group, Campus Drie Eiken, Universiteitsplein 1, B-2610 Wilrijk, Belgium

10.1163/1568539X-00003278
/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003278
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
10
5
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003278
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Amrhein V. , Johannessen L.E. , Kristiansen L. , Slagsvold T. ( 2008). "Reproductive strategy and singing activity: blue tit and great tit compared". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 62: 1633- 1641. [Crossref]
2. Amy M. , Sprau P. , de Goede P. , Naguib M. ( 2010). "Effects of personality on territory defence in communication networks: a playback experiment with radio-tagged great tits". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 277: 3685- 3692. [Crossref]
3. Bell A.M. ( 2007). "Future directions in behavioural syndromes research". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 274: 755- 761. [Crossref]
4. Bell A.M. , Hankison S.J. , Laskowski K.L. ( 2009). "The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis". — Anim. Behav. Vol 77: 771- 783. [Crossref]
5. Boeckle M. , Bugnyar T. ( 2012). "Long-term memory for affiliates in ravens". — Curr. Biol. Vol 22: 801- 806. [Crossref]
6. Chew S.J. , Mello C. , Nottebohm F. , Jarvis E. , Vicario D.S. ( 1995). "Decrements in auditory responses to a repeated conspecific song are long-lasting and require two periods of protein synthesis in the songbird forebrain". — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol 92: 3406- 3410. [Crossref]
7. Conway C.J. ( 2011). "Standardized North American marsh bird monitoring protocol". — Waterbirds Vol 34: 319- 346. [Crossref]
8. Deecke V.B. , Slater P.J.B. , Ford J.K.B. ( 2002). "Selective habituation shapes acoustic predator recognition in harbour seals". — Nature Vol 420: 171- 173. [Crossref]
9. Dong S. , Clayton D.F. ( 2009). "Habituation in songbirds". — Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. Vol 92: 183- 188. [Crossref]
10. Dong S. , Replogle K.L. , Hasadsri L. , Imai B.S. , Yau P.M. , Rodriguez-Zas S. , Southey B.R. , Sweedler J.V. , Clayton D.F. ( 2009). "Discrete molecular states in the brain accompany changing responses to a vocal signal". — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol 106: 11364- 11369. [Crossref]
11. Douglas S.B. , Mennill D.J. ( 2010). "A review of acoustic playback techniques for studying avian vocal duets". — J. Field Ornithol. Vol 81: 115- 129. [Crossref]
12. Falls J.B. ( 1992). "Playback — a historical-perspective". — In: Playback and studies of animals communication: problems and prospects( McGregor P.K. , ed.). Plenum, New York, NY, p.  11- 33. [Crossref]
13. Garamszegi L.Z. , Calhim S. , Dochtermann N. , Hegyi G. , Hurd P.L. , Jorgensen C. , Kutsukake N. , Lajeunesse M.J. , Pollard K.A. , Schielzeth H. , Symonds M.R.E. , Nakagawa S. ( 2009). "Changing philosophies and tools for statistical inferences in behavioral ecology". — Behav. Ecol. Vol 20: 1363- 1375. [Crossref]
14. Garamszegi L.Z. , Eens M. , Torok J. ( 2008). "Birds reveal their personality when singing". — PLoS ONE Vol 3: e2647. [Crossref]
15. Godard R. ( 1991). "Long-term memory of individual neighbors in a migratory songbird". — Nature Vol 350: 228- 229. [Crossref]
16. Hemmi J.M. , Merkle T. ( 2009). "High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 276: 4381- 4388. [Crossref]
17. Krebs J.R. , Ashcroft R. , Webber M. ( 1978). "Song repertoires and territory defence in the great tit". — Nature Vol 271: 539- 542. [Crossref]
18. Krebs J.R. ( 1976). "Habituation and song repertoires in great tit". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 1: 215- 227. [Crossref]
19. Krebs J.R. , Ashcroft R. , Vanorsdol K. ( 1981). "Song matching in the great tit Parus majorL."— Anim. Behav. Vol 29: 918- 923. [Crossref]
20. Lambrechts M.M. ( 1992). "Male quality and playback in the great tit". — In: Playback and studies of animals communication: problems and prospects( McGregor P.K. , ed.). Plenum, New York, NY, p.  135- 152. [Crossref]
21. Lee W. , Lee S.-I. , Choe J. , Jablonski P. ( 2011). "Wild birds recognize individual humans: experiments on magpies, Pica pica ". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 14: 817- 825. [Crossref]
22. Lessells C.M. , Boag P.T. ( 1987). "Unrepeatable repeatabilities: a common mistake". — Auk Vol 104: 116- 121. [Crossref]
23. Levey D.J. , Londono G.A. , Ungvari-Martin J. , Hiersoux M.R. , Jankowski J.E. , Poulsen J.R. , Stracey C.M. , Robinson S.K. ( 2009). "Urban mockingbirds quickly learn to identify individual humans". — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol 106: 8959- 8962. [Crossref]
24. Linhart P. , Fuchs R. , Polakova S. , Slabbekoorn H. ( 2012). "Once bitten twice shy: long-term behavioural changes caused by trapping experience in willow warblers Phylloscopus trochilus ". — J. Avian Biol. Vol 43: 186- 192. [Crossref]
25. McGregor P.K. , Avery M.I. ( 1986). "The unsung songs of great tits ( Parus major) — learning neighbors songs for discrimination". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 18: 311- 316. [Crossref]
26. McGregor P.K. , Horn A.G. ( 1992). "Strophe length and response to playback in great tits". — Anim. Behav. Vol 43: 667- 676. [Crossref]
27. McGregor P.K. , Krebs J.R. ( 1982). "Song types in a population of great tits ( Parus major) — their distribution, abundance and acquisition by individuals". — Behaviour Vol 79: 126- 152. [Crossref]
28. McGregor P.K. , Krebs J.R. , Perrins C.M. ( 1981). "Song repertoires and lifetime reproductive success in the great tit ( Parus major)". — Am. Nat. Vol 118: 149- 159. [Crossref]
29. Nakagawa S. , Schielzeth H. ( 2010). "Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: a practical guide for biologists". — Biol. Rev. Vol 85: 935- 956.
30. Peake T.M. , Terry A.M.R. , McGregor P.K. , Dabelsteen T. ( 2001). "Male great tits eavesdrop on simulated male-to-male vocal interactions". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 268: 1183- 1187. [Crossref]
31. Peake T.M. , Terry A.M.R. , McGregor P.K. , Dabelsteen T. ( 2002). "Do great tits assess rivals by combining direct experience with information gathered by eavesdropping?"— Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 269: 1925- 1929. [Crossref]
32. Peeke H.V.S. ( 1984). "Habituation and the maintenance of territorial boundaries". — In: Habituation, sesitization, and behavior( Peeke H. , Petrinovich L. , eds). Academic Press, New York, NY, p.  393- 421.
33. Rankin C.H. , Abrams T. , Barry R.J. , Bhatnagar S. , Clayton D.F. , Colombo J. , Coppola G. , Geyer M.A. , Glanzman D.L. , Marsland S. , McSweeney F.K. , Wilson D.A. , Wu C.-F. , Thompson R.F. ( 2009). "Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation". — Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. Vol 92: 135- 138. [Crossref]
34. Rivera-Gutierrez H.F. , Pinxten R. , Eens M. ( 2010). "Multiple signals for multiple messages: great tit, Parus major, song signals age and survival". — Anim. Behav. Vol 80: 451- 459. [Crossref]
35. Rivera-Gutierrez H.F. , Pinxten R. , Eens M. ( 2011a). "Difficulties when assessing birdsong learning programmes under field conditions: a re-evaluation of song repertoire flexibility in the great tit". — PLoS ONE Vol 6: e16003. [Crossref]
36. Rivera-Gutierrez H.F. , Pinxten R. , Eens M. ( 2011b). "Songs differing in consistency elicit differential aggressive response in territorial birds". — Biol. Lett. Vol 7: 339- 342. [Crossref]
37. Rivera-Gutierrez H.F. , Pinxten R. , Eens M. ( 2012). "Tuning and fading voices in songbirds: age-dependent changes in two acoustic traits across the life span". — Anim. Behav. Vol 83: 1279- 1283. [Crossref]
38. Searcy W.A. , Anderson R.C. , Nowicki S. ( 2006). "Bird song as a signal of aggressive intent". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 60: 234- 241. [Crossref]
39. Searcy W.A. , Beecher M.D. ( 2009). "Song as an aggressive signal in songbirds". — Anim. Behav. Vol 78: 1281- 1292. [Crossref]
40. Sih A. , Bell A. , Johnson J.C. ( 2004). "Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview". — Trends Ecol. Evol. Vol 19: 372- 378. [Crossref]
41. Temeles E.J. ( 1994). "The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they ‘dear enemies’?"— Anim. Behav. Vol 47: 339- 350. [Crossref]
42. Tibbetts E.A. , Dale J. ( 2007). "Individual recognition: it is good to be different". — Trends Ecol. Evol. Vol 22: 529- 537. [Crossref]
43. Van Duyse E. , Pinxten R. , Eens M. ( 2000). "Does testosterone affect the trade-off between investment in sexual/territorial behaviour and parental care in male great tits?"— Behaviour Vol 137: 1503- 1515. [Crossref]
44. Ydenberg R.C. , Giraldeau L.A. , Falls J.B. ( 1988). "Neighbours, strangers, and the asymmetric war of attrition". — Anim. Behav. Vol 36: 343- 347. [Crossref]
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003278
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003278
2015-06-04
2018-09-23

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation