Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Differences in short-term vocal learning in parrots, a comparative study

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

Parrots are renowned for their vocal learning abilities. Yet only few parrot species have been investigated and empirically proven to possess vocal learning abilities. The aim of this study was to investigate if short-term vocal learning may be a widespread phenomenon among Psittaciformes. Through an interactive experiment we compare the ability of four parrot species, the peach-fronted conure ( Aratinga aurea), the cockatiel ( Nymphicus hollandicus), the peach-faced lovebird ( Agapornis roseicollis) and the budgerigar ( Melopsittacus undulatus), to vocally match playback of contact calls. All four species made an overall change to their contact call in response to the playback, and they also varied the degree of similarity with the playback call throughout the playback experiment. The peach-fronted conure showed the biggest overall changes to their contact calls by vocally matching the playback call and the budgerigar showed the least change. The cockatiel and the peach-faced lovebird showed intermediary levels of change making their calls overall less similar to the playback call. The peach-fronted conure responded with highest similarity to familiar individuals and the cockatiel responded with an overall higher similarity to female playback stimuli. Cockatiel males and budgerigar males responded with a higher call rate to playback than female conspecifics. Peach-faced lovebirds responded fastest to unfamiliar males. Based on the results we conclude that short-term vocal learning is a widespread phenomenon among parrots. The way short-term vocal learning is used however, differs between species suggesting that short-term vocal learning have different functions in different species.

Affiliations: 1: aBehavioural Ecology Group, Department of Biology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark; 2: bDepartment of Bioscience, Aarhus University, Vejlsøvej 25, 8600 Silkeborg, Denmark

10.1163/1568539X-00003286
/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003286
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
10
5
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003286
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Aubin T. , Mathevon N. , Staszewski V. , Boulinier T. ( 2007). "Acoustic communication in the kittiwake Rissa tridactyla: potential cues for sexual and individual signatures in long calls". — Polar Biol. Vol 30: 1027- 1033. [Crossref]
2. Balsby T.J.S. , Adams D.M. ( 2011). "Vocal similarity and familiarity determine response to potential flockmates in orange-fronted conures (Psittacidae)". — Anim. Behav. Vol 81: 983- 991. [Crossref]
3. Balsby T.J.S. , Bradbury J.W. ( 2009). "Vocal matching by orange-fronted conures ( Aratinga canicularis)". — Behav. Process. Vol 82: 133- 139. [Crossref]
4. Balsby T.J.S. , Scarl J.C. ( 2008). "Sex-specific responses to vocal convergence and divergence of contact calls in orange-fronted conures ( Aratinga canicularis)". — Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Vol 275: 2147- 2154. [Crossref]
5. Balsby T.J.S. , Momberg V.M. , Dabelsteen T. ( 2012). "Vocal imitation in parrots allows addressing individuals in a dynamic communication network". — Plos One Vol 7: e49747. [Crossref]
6. Bartlett P. , Slater P.J.B. ( 1999). "The effect of new recruits on the flock specific call of budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus)". — Ethol. Ecol. Evol. Vol 11: 139- 147. [Crossref]
7. Berg K.S. , Delgado S. , Okawa R. , Beissinger S.R. , Bradbury J.W. ( 2011). "Contact calls are used for individual mate recognition in free-ranging green-rumped parrotlets, Forpus passerinus ". — Anim. Behav. Vol 81: 241- 248. [Crossref]
8. Boughman J.W. ( 1997). "Greater spear-nosed bats give group-distinctive calls". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 40: 61- 70. [Crossref]
9. Bradbury J.W. ( 2003). "Vocal communication in wild parrots". — In: Animal social complexity: intelligence, culture and individualized societies( deWaal F.B.M. , Tyack P.L. , eds). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, p.  239- 316.
10. Bradbury J.W. , Cortopassi K.A. , Clemmons J.R. , Kroodsma D. ( 2001). "Geographical variation in the contact calls of orange-fronted parakeets". — Auk Vol 118: 958- 972. [Crossref]
11. Brockway B.F. ( 1964). "Ethological studies of the budgerigar ( Melopsittacus undulatus): non-reproductive behavior". — Behaviour Vol 22: 193- 222. [Crossref]
12. Brown S.D. , Dooling R.J. , Grady R.O. ( 1988). "Perceptual organization of acoustic stimuli by budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus). III. Contact calls". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 102: 236- 247. [Crossref]
13. Buhrman-Deever S.C. , Hobson E.A. , Hobson A.D. ( 2008). "Individual recognition and selective response to contact calls in foraging brown-throated conures, Aratinga pertinax ". — Anim. Behav. Vol 76: 1715- 1725. [Crossref]
14. Catchpole C.K. ( 1983). "Variation in the song of the great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceusin relation to mate attraction and territorial defence". — Anim. Behav. Vol 31: 1217- 1225. [Crossref]
15. Catchpole C.K. , Slater P.J.B. ( 2008). Bird song: biological themes and variations. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. [Crossref]
16. Charrier I. , Mathevon N. , Jouventin P. ( 2003). "Vocal signature recognition of mothers by fur seal pups". — Anim. Behav. Vol 65: 543- 550. [Crossref]
17. Cortopassi K.A. , Bradbury J.W. ( 2000). "The comparison of harmonically rich sounds using spectrographic cross-correlation and principal coordinates analysis". — Bioacoustics Vol 11: 89- 127. [Crossref]
18. Cortopassi K.A. , Bradbury J.W. ( 2006). "Contact call diversity in wild orange-fronted parakeet pairs, Aratinga canicularis ". — Anim. Behav. Vol 71: 1141- 1154. [Crossref]
19. Dabelsteen T. , McGregor P.K. ( 1996). "Dynamic acoustic communication and interactive playback". — In: Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds( Kroodsma D.E. , Miller E.H. , eds). Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, p.  377- 397.
20. Dabelsteen T. , McGregor P.K. , Lampe H.M. , Langmore N.E. , Holland J. ( 1998). "Quiet song in song birds: an overlooked phenomenon". — Bioacoustics Vol 9: 89- 105. [Crossref]
21. de Kort S.R. , Eldermire E.R.B. , Cramer E.R.A. , Vehrencamp S. ( 2009). "The deterrent effect of bird song in territory defense". — Behav. Ecol. Vol 20: 200- 206. [Crossref]
22. Delsaut M. , Roy J.C. ( 1980). "Auditory and visual stimuli as reinforcers among lovebirds ( Agapornis roseicollis)". — Behav. Neurol. Biol. Vol 28: 319- 334. [Crossref]
23. Dilger W.C. ( 1960). "The comparative ethology of the african parrot genus Agapornis ". — Z. Tierpsychol. Vol 17: 649- 685. [Crossref]
24. Dyrcz A. , Sauer-Gurth H. , Tkadlec E. , Wink M. ( 2004). "Offspring sex ratio variation in relation to brood size and mortality in a promiscuous species: the aquatic warbler Acrocephalus paludicola ". — Ibis Vol 146: 269- 280. [Crossref]
25. Eriksson D. , Wallin L. ( 1986). "Male bird song attracts females — a field experiment". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 19: 297- 299. [Crossref]
26. Farabaugh S.M. , Dooling R.J. ( 1996). "Acoustic communication in parrots: laboratory and field studies of budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus ". — In: Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds( Kroodsma D.E. , Miller E.H. , eds). Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, p.  97- 117.
27. Farabaugh S.M. , Linzenbold A. , Dooling R.J. ( 1994). "Vocal plasticity in budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus): evidence for social factors in the learning of contact calls". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 108: 81- 92. [Crossref]
28. Freeberg T.M. , Dunbar R.I.M. , Ord T.J. ( 2012). "Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity". — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 367: 1785- 1801. [Crossref]
29. Griffiths R. , Double M.C. , Orr K. , Dawson R.J.G. ( 1998). "A DNA test to sex most birds". — Mol. Ecol. Vol 7: 1071- 1075. [Crossref]
30. Hardy J.W. ( 1965). "Flock social behavior of the orange-fronted parakeet". — Condor Vol 67: 140- 156. [Crossref]
31. Hile A.G. , Plummer T.K. , Striedter G.F. ( 2000). "Male vocal imitation produces call convergence during pair bonding in budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus ". — Anim. Behav. Vol 59: 1209- 1218. [Crossref]
32. Hile A.G. , Striedter G.F. ( 2000). "Call convergence within groups of female budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus)". — Ethology Vol 106: 1105- 1114. [Crossref]
33. Hultsch H. , Todt D. ( 2004). "Learning to sing". — In: Nature’s music( Marler P. , Slabbekoorn H. , eds). Elsevier, San Diego, CA, p.  80- 107. [Crossref]
34. Janik V.M. , Slater P.J.B. ( 1998). "Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls". — Anim. Behav. Vol 56: 829- 838. [Crossref]
35. Jouventin P. , Aubin T. , Lengagne T. ( 1999). "Finding a parent in a king penguin colony: the acoustic system of individual recognition". — Anim. Behav. Vol 57: 1175- 1183. [Crossref]
36. Juniper T. , Parr M. ( 1998). Parrots: a guide to parrots of the world. — Yale University Press, London.
37. Kondo N. , Watanabe S. ( 2009). "Contact call: information and social function". — Jpn. Psychol. Res. Vol 51: 197- 208. [Crossref]
38. Krebs J.R. ( 1976). "Bird song and territory defense". — New Scientist Vol 70: 534- 536.
39. Kroodsma D.E. ( 1989). "Suggested experimental designs for song playbacks". — Anim. Behav. Vol 37: 600- 609. [Crossref]
40. Littell R.C. , Milliken G.A. , Stroup W.W. , Wolfinger R.D. , Schabenberger O. ( 2006). SAS for mixed models. — SAS Institute, Cary, NC.
41. Marler P. ( 2004). "Bird calls: a cornucopia for communication". — In: Nature’s music( Slabbekorn P.M.H. , ed.). Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, CA, p.  132- 177. [Crossref]
42. Martella M.B. , Bucher E.H. ( 1990). "Vocalizations of the monk parakeet". — Bird Behav. Vol 8: 101- 110. [Crossref]
43. Moravec M.L. , Striedter G.F. , Burley N.T. ( 2006). "Assortative pairing based on contact call similarity in budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus ". — Ethology Vol 112: 1108- 1116. [Crossref]
44. Park T.J. , Dooling R.J. ( 1985). "Perception of species-specific contact calls by budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus)". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 99: 391- 402. [Crossref]
45. Rendall D. , Cheney D.L. , Seyfarth R.M. ( 2000). "Proximate factors mediating “contact” calls in adult female baboons ( Papio cynocephalus ursinus) and their infants". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 114: 36- 46. [Crossref]
46. Scarl J.C. , Bradbury J.W. ( 2009). "Rapid vocal convergence in an Australian cockatoo, the galah Eolophus roseicapillus ". — Anim. Behav. Vol 77: 1019- 1026. [Crossref]
47. Seibert L.M. , Crowell-Davis S.L. ( 2001). "Gender effects on aggression, dominance rank, and affiliative behaviors in a flock of captive adult cockatiels ( Nymphicus hollandicus)". — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. Vol 71: 155- 170. [Crossref]
48. Sinclair I. , Hockey P. , Tarboton W. ( 2002). Sasol birds of Southern Africa. — Struick, Cape Town.
49. Stokes A.W. ( 1967). "Behavior of the bobwhite, Colinus virginianus ". — Auk Vol 84: 1- 33. [Crossref]
50. Thomsen H.M. , Balsby T.J.S. , Dabelsteen T. ( 2013). "Individual variation in the contact calls of the monomorphic peach-fronted conure, Aratinga aurea, and its potential role in communication". — Bioacoustics Vol 22: 215- 227. [Crossref]
51. Vehrencamp S.L. , Ritter A.F. , Keever M. , Bradbury J.W. ( 2003). "Responses to playback of local vs. distant contact calls in the orange-fronted conure, Aratinga canicularis ". — Ethology Vol 109: 37- 54. [Crossref]
52. Vignal C. , Mathevon N. , Mottin S. ( 2004). "Audience drives male songbird response to partner’s voice". — Nature Vol 430: 448- 451. [Crossref]
53. Wanker R. , Apcin J. , Jennerjahn B. , Waibel B. ( 1998). "Discrimination of different social companions in spectacled parrotlets ( Forpus conspicillatus): evidence for individual vocal recognition". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 43: 197- 202. [Crossref]
54. Wanker R. , Fischer J. ( 2001). "Intra- and interindividual variation in the contact calls of spectacled parrotlets ( Forpus conspicillatus)". — Behaviour Vol 138: 709- 726. [Crossref]
55. Wanker R. , Sugama Y. , Prinage S. ( 2005). "Vocal labelling of family members in spectacled parrotlets, Forpus conspicillatus ". — Anim. Behav. Vol 70: 111- 118. [Crossref]
56. Warburton L. , Perrin M.R. ( 2005). "Roosting requirements and behaviour of the black-cheeked lovebird, Agapornis nigrigenis, in Zambia". — Trop. Zool. Vol 18: 39- 48. [Crossref]
57. Wright T.F. ( 1996). "Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 263: 867- 872. [Crossref]
58. Wyndham E. ( 1980). "Diurnal cycle, behaviour and social organization of the budgerigar Melopsittacus undulatus ". — Emu Vol 80: 25- 33. [Crossref]
59. Zann R. ( 1965). Behavioral studies of the quarrion ( Nymphicus hollandicus). — University of New England, Zoology Department, Armidale, NSW.
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003286
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003286
2015-07-27
2018-07-17

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation