Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Stone handling, an object play behaviour in macaques: welfare and neurological health implications of a bio-culturally driven tradition

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

Object play in primates is viewed as generally having no immediate functional purpose, limited for the most part to immature individuals. At the proximate level, the occurrence of object play in immatures is regarded as being intrinsically self-rewarding, with the ultimate function of supporting motoneuronal development and the acquisition of skills necessary to prepare them for survival as adults. Stone handling (SH), a solitary object play behaviour occurs, and has been studied, in multiple free-ranging and captive troops of provisioned Japanese macaques, as well as rhesus and long-tailed macaques for over 35 years now. A review of our combined findings from these observations reveal that infants acquire SH in the first 3-4 months of life and exhibit increasingly more complex and varied behavioural patterns with age. The longitudinal data shows that many individuals maintain this activity throughout life, practicing it under relaxed ecological and social conditions. The ultimate function may be bimodal, promoting motor development in young and neural maintenance and regeneration in adult and aging individuals.

Affiliations: 1: aDepartment of Sociology and Anthropology, University of Sri Jayewardenepura, Nugegoda, Sri Lanka ; 2: bDepartment of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, 4401 University Drive W, Lethbridge, AB, Canada T1K 6T5 ; 3: cDepartment of Ecology and Social Behavior, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, 41-2 Kanrin, Inuyama, Aichi 484, Japan

*Corresponding author’s e-mail address: huffman.michael.8n@kyoto-u.ac.jp
10.1163/1568539X-00003361
/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003361
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
10
5
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003361
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Armand J. , Olivier E. , Edgley S.A. , Lemon R.N. (1996). "The postnatal development of corticospinal projections from motor cortex to the cervical enlargement in the macaque monkey". — J. Neurosci. Vol 17: 251-266.
2. Baldwin J.D. , Baldwin J.I. (1977). "The role of learning phenomena in the ontogeny of exploration and play". — In: Primate bio-social development ( Chevalier-Skolnikoff S. , Poirier F.E. , eds). Garland, New York, NY, p.  343-406.
3. Barber N. (1991). "Play and energy regulation in mammals". — Q. Rev. Biol. Vol 66: 129-147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/417142
4. Bateson P. , Mendl M. , Feaver J. (1990). "Play in the domestic cat is enhanced by rationing of the mother during lactation". — Anim. Behav. Vol 40: 514-525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80532-9
5. Bekoff M. (1995). "Play signals as punctuation: the structure of social play in canids". — Behaviour Vol 132: 419-429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853995X00649
6. Bekoff M. , Byers J.A. (1981). "A critical reanalysis of the ontogeny and phylogeny of mammalian social and locomotor play: an ethological hornets nest". — In: Behavioral development ( Immelmann K. , Barlow G. , Main M. , Petrinovich L. , eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  296-337.
7. Bell H.C. , McCaffrey D. , Forgie M.L. , Kolb B. , Pellis S.M. (2009). "The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the play fighting in rats". — Behav. Neurosci. Vol 123: 1158-1168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017617
8. Biben M. (1982). "Object play and social treatment of prey in bush dogs and crab-eating foxes". — Behaviour Vol 79: 201-211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853982X00256
9. Boissy A. , Manteuffel G. , Jensen M.B. , Oppermann M.R. , Spruijt B. , Keeling L.J. , Winckler C. , Forkan B. , Dimitrov I. , Langbein J. , Bakken M. , Veisser I. , Aubert A. (2007). "Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare". — Physiol. Behav. Vol 92: 375-397. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003
10. Bortoff G.A. , Strick P.L. (1993). "Corticospinal terminations in two New World primates: further evidence that corticoneuronal connections provide part of the neural substrate for manual dexterity". — J. Neurosci. Vol 13: 5105-5118.
11. Bourgeois J.-P. , Goldman-Rakic P.S. , Rakic P. (1994). "Synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys". — Cereb. Cortex Vol 4: 78-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/4.1.78
12. Burghardt G.M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: testing the limits. — MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
13. Burton F.D. , Bick M.J.A. (1972). "A drift in time can define a deme: the implications of tradition drift in primate societies for hominid evolution". — J. Hum. Evol. Vol 1: 53-59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0047-2484(72)90042-5
14. Byers J.A. , Walker C. (1995). "Refining the motor training hypothesis for the evolution of play". — Am. Nat. Vol 146: 25-40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/285785
15. Cotman C.W. , Berchtold N.C. (2002). "Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity". — Trends Neurosci. Vol 25: 295-301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02143-4
16. Coyle J.T. (2003). "Use it or lose it — do effortful mental activities protect against dementia?" — New Engl. J. Med. Vol 348: 2489-2490. http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp030051
17. Darwish M. , Korányi L. , Nyakas C. , Ferenz A. (2001). "Induced social interactions reduces corticosterone stress response to anxiety in adult and aged rats". — Klin. Kisérl. Lab. Med. Vol 28: 108-111.
18. Dawkins M.S. (2006). "A user’s guide to animal welfare science". — Trends Ecol. Evol. Vol 21: 77-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2005.10.017
19. de Waal F.B.M. (2001). The ape and the sushi-master: cultural reflections of a primatologist. — Basic Books, New York, NY.
20. Dean L. , Vale G. , Laland K.N. , Flynn E. , Kendal R. (2014). "Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective". — Biol. Rev. Vol 89: 284-301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/brv.12053
21. Fagen R. (1981). Animal play behavior. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.
22. Fairbanks L.A. (2000). "The developmental timing of primate play: a neural selection model". — In: Biology, brains and behavior: the evolution of human development ( Parker S.T. , Langer J. , McKinney M.L. , eds). School of American Research Press, Oxford, p.  131-158.
23. Fedigan L. (1972). "Social and solitary play in a colony of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops)". — Primates Vol 13: 347-364. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01793655
24. Fragaszy D.M. , Adams-Curtis L.E. (1991). "Generative aspects of manipulation in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 105: 387-397. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.105.4.387
25. Fraser D. , Duncan I.J.H. (1998). "‘Pleasures’, ‘pains’ and animal welfare: toward a natural history of affect". — Anim. Welf. Vol 7: 383-396.
26. Galea M.P. , Darian-Smith I. (1995). "Postnatal maturation of the direct corticospinal projections in the macaque monkey". — Cereb. Cortex Vol 5: 518-540. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/5.6.518
27. Göncü A. , Gaskins S. (2007). Play and development: evolutionary, sociocultural, and functional perspectives. — Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, London.
28. Graham K.L. , Burghardt G.M. (2010). "Current perspectives on the biological study of play: signs of progress". — Q. Rev. Biol. Vol 85: 393-418. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/656903
29. Groos K. (1898). The play of animals. — D. Appleton, New York, NY. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12894-000
30. Groos K. (1901). The play of man. — D. Appleton, New York, NY. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/13084-000
31. Gumert M.D. , Kluck M. , Malaivijitnond S. (2007). "The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone age and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 71: 594-608. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20694
32. Gumert M.D. , Malaivijitnond S. (2012). "Marine prey processed with stone tools by Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) in intertidal habitats". — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. Vol 149: 447-457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.22143
33. Hall G.S. (1904). Adolescence: its psychology and its relations to physiology, anthropology, sociology, sex, crime, religion and education. — D. Appleton, New York, NY.
34. Hall S.L. (1998). "Object play by adult animals". — In: Animal play: evolutionary, comparative and ecological perspectives ( Bekoff M. , Byers J.A. , eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  45-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511608575.004
35. Hauser M.D. (1988). "Invention and social transmission: new data from wild vervet monkeys". — In: Machiavelian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans ( Byrne R.W. , Whiten A. , eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p.  327-343.
36. Held S.D.E. , Spinka M. (2011). "Animal play and animal welfare". — Anim. Behav. Vol 81: 891-899. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007
37. Hiraiwa M. (1975). "Pebble-collecting behavior by juvenile Japanese monkeys". — Monkey Vol 19: 24-25 (in Japanese).
38. Hof P.R. , Duan H. (2001). "Age-related morphologic alteration in the brain of Old World and New World anthropoid monkeys". — In: Functional neurobiology of aging ( Hof P.R. , Mobbs C.V. , eds). Academic Press, San Diego, CA, p.  435-445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-012351830-9/50030-5
39. Huffman M.A. (1984). "Stone-play of Macaca fuscata in Arashiyama B troop: transmission of a non-adaptive behavior". — J. Hum. Evol. Vol 13: 725-735. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(84)80022-6
40. Huffman M.A. (1991). "History of Arashiyama Japanese macaques in Kyoto, Japan". — In: The monkeys of Arashiyama: thirty-five years of research in Japan and the West ( Fedigan L.M. , Asquith P.J. , eds). SUNY Press, Albany, NY, p.  21-53.
41. Huffman M.A. (1996). "Acquisition of innovative cultural behaviors in non-human primates: a case study of stone handling, a socially transmitted behavior in Japanese macaques". — In: Social learning in animals: the roots of culture ( Galef B. , Heyes C. , eds). Academic Press, Orlando, FL, p.  267-289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-012273965-1/50014-5
42. Huffman M.A. , Quiatt D. (1986). "Stone handling by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): implications for tool use of stones". — Primates Vol 27: 413-423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02381887
43. Huffman M.A. , Hirata S. (2003). "Biological and ecological foundations of primate behavioral tradition". — In: The biology of tradition: models and evidence ( Fragaszy D.M. , Perry S. , eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  267-296. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511584022.011
44. Huffman M.A. , Nahallage C.A.D. , Leca J.-B. (2008). "Cultured monkeys, social learning cast in stones". — Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. Vol 17: 410-414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00616.x
45. Huffman M.A. , Leca J.-B. , Nahallage C.A.D. (2010). "Cultured Japanese macaques: a multidisciplinary approach to stone handling behavior and its implications for the evolution of behavioral tradition in nonhuman primates". — In: The Japanese macaques ( Nakagawa N. , Nakamichi M. , Sugiura H. , eds). Springer, Tokyo, p.  191-219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-53886-8_9
46. Jaman F.M. , Huffman M.A. (2008). "Enclosure environment affects the activity budgets of captive Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 70: 1133-1144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20612
47. Jennings P.J. , Janowsky J.S. , Orwoll E. (1998). "Estrogen and sequential movement". — Behav. Neurosci. Vol 112: 154-159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.112.1.154
48. Joseph J.A. , Kochman K. , Roth G.S. (1989). "Reduction of motor behavioural deficits in senescence via chronic prolactin or estrogen administration: time course and putative mechanisms of action". — Brain Res. Vol 505: 195-202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(89)91442-X
49. Kerckhoff A.C. (2002). "A theory of hysterical contagion". — Hum. Nat. Coll. Behav. Vol 21: 81-93.
50. Kritzer M.F. , Kohama S.G. (1998). "Ovarian hormones influence the morphology, distribution, and density of tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactive axons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of adult rhesus monkeys". — J. Comp. Neurol. Vol 395: 1-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19980525)395:1<1::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-4
51. Kummer H. , Goodall J. (1985). "Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates". — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 308: 203-214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1985.0020
52. Kuypers H.G.J.M. (1981). "Anatomy of the descending pathways". — In: Handbook of physiology: the nervous system II ( Brookhart J.M. , Mountcastle V.B. , eds). American Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD, p.  597-666.
53. Lacreuse A. , Diehl M.M. , Goh M.Y. , Hall M.J. , Volk A.M. , Chhabra R.K. , Herndon J.G. (2005). "Sex differences in age-related motor slowing in the rhesus monkey: behavioral and neuroimaging data". — Neurobiol. Aging Vol 26: 543-551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.05.007
54. Laurin D. , Verreault M.D. , Lindsay J. , MacPherson K. , Rockwood K. (2001). "Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons". — Arch. Neurol. Vol 58: 498-504. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archneur.58.3.498
55. Lawrence A. (1987). "Consumer demand theory and the assessment of animal welfare". — Anim. Behav. Vol 35: 293-295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80236-1
56. Lawrence D.G. , Hopkins D.A. (1976). "The development of motor control in rhesus monkeys: evidence concerning the role of corticomotoneuronal connections". — Brain Vol 99: 235-254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/99.2.235
57. Leca J.-B. (2012). "Integrative approach to cultural behaviour: the case of stone handling in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)". — Techn. Cult. Vol 57: 142-163 (in French).
58. Leca J.-B. (2015). "Arbitrary cultural behavior patterns are not unique to humans". [Invited commentary on Nakagawa et al.]Curr. Anthropol. Vol 56: 112-113.
59. Leca J.-B. , Gunst N. , Huffman M.A. (2007a). "Age-related differences in the performance, diffusion, and maintenance of stone handling, a behavioral tradition in Japanese macaques". — J. Hum. Evol. Vol 53: 691-708. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.05.009
60. Leca J.-B. , Gunst N. , Huffman M.A. (2007b). "Japanese macaque cultures: inter- and intra-troop behavioural variability of stone handling patterns across 10 troops". — Behaviour Vol 144: 251-281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853907780425712
61. Leca J.-B. , Gunst N. , Huffman M.A. (2008a). "Of stones and monkeys: testing ecological constraints on stone handling, a behavioral tradition in Japanese macaques". — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. Vol 135: 233-244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20726
62. Leca J.-B. , Gunst N. , Huffman M.A. (2008b). "Food provisioning and stone handling tradition in Japanese macaques: a comparative study of ten troops". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 70: 803-813. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20551
63. Leca J.-B. , Gunst N. , Huffman M.A. (2010). "Indirect social influence in the maintenance of the stone handling tradition in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)". — Anim. Behav. Vol 79: 117-126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.035
64. Leca J.-B. , Gunst N. , Huffman M.A. (2011). "Complexity in object manipulation by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): a cross-sectional analysis of manual coordination in stone handling patterns". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 125: 61-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020868
65. Leca J.-B. , Gunst N. , Huffman M.A. (2012). "Thirty years of stone handling tradition in Arashiyama-Kyoto macaques: implications for cumulative culture and tool use in non-human primates". — In: The monkeys of Stormy Mountain: 60 years of primatological research on the Japanese macaques of Arashiyama ( Leca J.-B. , Huffman M.A. , Vasey P.L. , eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  223-257.
66. Leca J.-B. , Gunst N. , Ottenheimer Carrier L. , Vasey P.L. (2014). "Inter-group variation in non-conceptive sexual activity in female Japanese macaques: could it be cultural?" — Anim. Behav. Cogn. Vol 1: 387-409. http://dx.doi.org/10.12966/abc.08.12.2014
67. Lewis K.P. , Barton R.A. (2006). "Amygdala size and hypothalamus size predict social play frequency in non-human primates: a comparative analysis using independent contrasts". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 120: 31-37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.120.1.31
68. Lockman J. (2000). "A perception–action perspective on tool use development". — Child Dev. Vol 71: 137-144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00127
69. Matsuzawa T. , Biro D. , Humle T. , Inoue-Nakamura N. , Tonooka R. , Yamakoshi G. (2001). "Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master-apprenticeship". — In: Primate origins of human cognition and behaviour ( Matsuzawa T. , ed.). Springer, Tokyo, p.  557-574. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-09423-4
70. McGrew W.C. , Collins D.A. (1985). "Tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) to obtain termites (Macrotermes herus) in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 9: 47-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350090106
71. Mitchell R.W. (2002). Pretending in animals, children, and adult humans. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511542282
72. Mustoe A.C. , Taylor J.H. , Birnie A.K. , Huffman M.C. , French J.A. (2014). "Gestational cortisol and social play shape development of marmosets’ HPA functioning and behavioral responses to stressors". — Dev. Psychobiol. Vol 56: 1229-1243.
73. Nahallage C.A.D. , Huffman M.A. (2007a). "Acquisition and development of stone handling behavior in infant Japanese macaques". — Behaviour Vol 144: 1193-1215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853907781890959
74. Nahallage C.A.D. , Huffman M.A. (2007b). "Age-specific functions of stone handling, a solitary-object play behavior, in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 69: 267-281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20348
75. Nahallage C.A.D. , Huffman M.A. (2008a). "Proximate factors associated with the occurrence of stone handling behavior in a captive troop of Japanese macaques". — Int. J. Primatol. Vol 29: 795-806. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10764-008-9268-3
76. Nahallage C.A.D. , Huffman M.A. (2008b). "Comparison of stone handling behavior in two macaque species: implications for the role of phylogeny and environment in primate cultural variation". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 70: 1124-1132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20610
77. Nahallage C.A.D. , Huffman M.A. (2012). "Stone handling behavior in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), a behavioral propensity for solitary object play shared with Japanese macaques". — Primates Vol 53: 71-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10329-011-0279-x
78. Norscia I. , Palagi E. (2001). "When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy and possible stress reduction in common marmosets". — Primates Vol 52: 101-104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10329-010-0228-0
79. Nunes S. , Muecke E.-M. , Anthony J.A. , Batterbee A.S. (1999). "Endocrine and energetic mediation of play behavior in free-ranging Belding’s ground squirrels". — Horm. Behav. Vol 36: 153-165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.1999.1538
80. Olivier E. , Edgley S.A. , Armand J. , Lemon R.N. (1997). "An electrophysiological study of the postnatal development of the corticospinal system in the macaque monkey". — J. Neurosci. Vol 17: 267-276.
81. Palagi E. , Cordoni G. , Borgonini Tarli S.M. (2004). "Immediate and delayed benefits of play behaviour: new evidence from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)". — Ethology Vol 110: 949-962. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.01035.x
82. Parker S.T. , Gibson K.R. (1977). "Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in Cebus monkeys and great apes". — J. Hum. Evol. Vol 6: 623-641. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(77)80135-8
83. Pellis S.M. (1991). "How motivationally distinct is play? A preliminary case study". — Anim. Behav. Vol 42: 851-853. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80129-0
84. Pellis S.M. , Pellis V.C. , Bell H.C. (2010). "The function of play in the development of the social brain". — Am. J. Play Vol 2: 278-296.
85. Pereira M.E. , Fairbanks L.A. (1993). Juvenile primates: life history, development, and behavior. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.
86. Phillips C.G. (1971). Evolution of the corticospinal tract in primates with special reference to the hand. — In: Proceedings of the 3rd international congress in primatology. Karger, Zurich, p. 22-23.
87. Poirier F.E. , Bellisari A. , Haines L. (1978). "Functions of primate play behavior". — In: Social play in primates ( Smith E.O. , ed.). Academic Press, San Diego, CA, p.  143-168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-652750-6.50011-0
88. Quiatt D. , Huffman M.A. (1993). "On home bases, nesting sites, activity centers, and new analytic perspectives". — Curr. Anthropol. Vol 34: 68-70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/204139
89. Rakic P. , Bourgeois J.-P. , Eckenhoff M.F. , Zecevic N. , Goldman-Rakic P.S. (1986). "Concurrent over-production of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex". — Science Vol 232: 232-235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.3952506
90. Ramsey J.K. , McGrew W.C. (2005). "Object play in great apes: studies in nature and captivity". — In: The nature of play: great apes and humans ( Pellegrini A.D. , Smith P.K. , eds). Guilford Press, New York, NY, p.  89-112.
91. Scheurer J. , Thierry B. (1985). "A further food-washing tradition in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)". — Primates Vol 26: 491-496. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02382463
92. Siviy S.M. (1998). "Neurobiological substrates of play behavior: glimpses into the structure and function of mammalian playfulness". — In: Animal play: evolutionary, comparative and ecological perspectives ( Bekoff M. , Byers J.A. , eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  221-242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511608575.012
93. Siviy S.M. (2010). "Play and adversity: how the playful mammalian brain withstands threats and anxieties". — Am. J. Play Vol 2: 297-314.
94. Smith E.O. (1978). "A historical view on the study of play: statement of the problem". — In: Social play in primates ( Smith E.O. , ed.). Academic Press, New York, NY, p.  1-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-652750-6.50006-7
95. Špinka M. , Newberry R.C. , Bekoff M. (2001). "Play: training for the unexpected". — Q. Rev. Biol. Vol 76: 141-168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/393866
96. Starin E.D. (1990). "Object manipulation by wild red colobus monkeys living in the Abuko Nature Reserve, the Gambia". — Primates Vol 31: 385-391. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02381109
97. Taylor J.H. , Mustoe A.C. , Hochfelder B. , French J.A. (2015). "Reunion behavior after social separation is associated with enhanced HPA recovery in young marmoset monkeys". — Psychoneuroendocrinology Vol 57: 93-101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.03.019
98. Thierry B. (1994). "Social transmission, tradition and culture in primates: from the epiphenomenon to the phenomenon". — Techn. Cult. Vol 23/24: 91-119.
99. Thierry B. (2007). "Unity in diversity: lessons from macaque societies". — Evol. Anthropol. Vol 16: 224-238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/evan.20147
100. Verghese J. , Lipton R.B. , Hall C.B. , Kuslansky G. , Katz M.J. (2003). "Low blood pressure and the risk of dementia in very old individuals". — Neurology Vol 61: 1667-1672. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/01.WNL.0000098934.18300.BE
101. Vig S. (2007). "Young children’s object play: a window on development". — J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. Vol 19: 201-215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10882-007-9048-6
102. Von Frijtag J.C. , Schot M. , van den Bos R. , Spruijt B.M. (2002). "Individual housing during the play period results in changed responses to and consequences of a psychosocial stress situation in rats". — Dev. Psychobiol. Vol 41: 58-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.10057
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003361
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003361
2016-07-07
2017-12-14

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation