Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

The baroque potheads: modification and embellishment in repeated call sequences of long-finned pilot whales

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

Vocal variation within calls that are generally stereotyped suggests multiple simultaneous functions. These vocal cues may be especially important for group-living species. We describe two fundamental call transition types within repeated call sequences of long-finned pilot whales (Globicephala melas): embellishment — discrete changes to a specific part of a call — and morphing — non-discrete small changes across a call. Of transitions between consecutive calls, 31% were embellished and 20% morphed. Modifications between pairs of consecutive calls were often followed by another modification of the same type, with sequences of embellished transitions generally alternating between ornamentation and simplification. Ten classes of embellishment varied in rate of occurrence as well as temporal location within a call. Most common were the addition/deletion of pulsed or tonal elements. Functions of these modifications could include conveying information on location or the emotional state of the signaller, or they could be products of vocal innovation.

Affiliations: 1: Department of Biology, Dalhousie University, 1355 Oxford Street, Halifax, NS, Canada B3H 4J1

*Corresponding author’s e-mail address: elizabeth.zwamborn@dal.ca
10.1163/1568539X-00003451
/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003451
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
10
5
Loading
Loading data from figshare Loading data from figshare
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003451
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Auger-Méthé M., Whitehead H. (2007). "The use of natural markings in the studies of long-finned pilot whales (Globicephala melas)". — Mar. Mammal Sci. Vol 23: 77-93. [Crossref]
2. Augusto J.F., Frasier T.R., Whitehead H. (2017). "Social structure of long-finned pilot whales (Globicephala melas) off northern Cape Breton Island, Nova Scotia". — Behaviour Vol 154: 509-540. [Crossref]
3. Bioacustics Research Program (2014). Raven Pro: interactive sound analysis software. The Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, available online at http://www.birds.cornell.edu/raven.
4. Boogert N.J., Giraldeau L.A., Lefebvre L. (2008). "Song complexity correlates with learning ability in zebra finch males". — Anim. Behav. Vol 76: 1735-1741. [Crossref]
5. Bradbury J.W., Vehrencamp S.L. (2011). Principles of animal communication. — Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA.
6. Busnel R.G., Dziedzic A. (1966). "Acoustic signals of the pilot whale Globicephala melaena and of the porpoises Delphinus delphis and Phocoena phocoena". — In: Whales, dolphins, and porpoises ( Norris K.S., ed.). University of California Press, Berkeley, CA, p.  607-646.
7. Caldwell M.C., Caldwell D., Tyack P.L. (1990). "Review of the signature-whistle hypothesis for the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin". — In: The bottlenose dolphin ( Leatherwood S., Reeves R.R., eds). Academic Press, San Diego, CA, p.  199-234. [Crossref]
8. Elowson A.M., Snowdon C.T., Lazaro-Perea C. (1998). "Infant “babbling” in a nonhuman primate: complex vocal sequences with repeated call types". — Behaviour Vol 135: 643-664. [Crossref]
9. Esch H.C., Sayigh L.S., Blum J.E., Wells R.S. (2009). "Whistles as potential indicators of stress in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)". — J. Mammal. Vol 90: 638-650. [Crossref]
10. Ford J.K.B. (1989). "Acoustic behaviour of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia". — Can. J. Zool. Vol 67: 727-745. [Crossref]
11. Ford J.K.B. (1991). "Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia". — Can. J. Zool. Vol 69: 1454-1483. [Crossref]
12. Fripp D., Owen C., Quintana-Rizzo E., Shapiro A., Buckstaff K., Jankowski K., Wells R., Tyack P.L. (2005). "Bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) calves appear to model their signature whistles on the signature whistles of community members". — Anim. Cogn. Vol 8: 17-26. [Crossref]
13. Gero S., Whitehead H., Rendell L. (2016). "Individual, unit, and vocal clan level identity cues in sperm whale codas". — Roy. Soc. Open Sci. Vol 3: 150372.
14. Hall M.L. (2004). "A review of the hypotheses for the functions of avian duetting". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 55: 415-430. [Crossref]
15. IBM (2013). IBM SPSS Statistics for Mac. IBM, Armonk, NY.
16. Janik V.M., King S.L., Sayigh L.S., Wells R.S. (2013). "Identifying signature whistles from recordings of groups of unrestrained bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)". — Mar. Mammal Sci. Vol 29: 109-122. [Crossref]
17. Janik V.M. (2000). "Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)". — Science Vol 289: 1355-1357. [Crossref]
18. Jensen F.H., Perez J.M., Johnson M., Soto N.A., Madsen P.T. (2011). "Calling under pressure: short-finned pilot whales make social calls during deep foraging dives". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 278: 3017-3025. [Crossref]
19. Kuczaj S.A., Makecha R., Trone M., Paulos R.D., Ramos J.A. (2006). "Role of peers in cultural innovation and cultural transmission: evidence from the play of dolphin calves". — Int. J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 19: 223-240.
20. Kummer H., Loy J. (1971). "Social behaviour and habitat (book review: Primate societies: group techniques of ecological adaptation)". — Science Vol 174: 49-51. [Crossref]
21. Lilly J.C. (1965). "Vocal mimicry in Tursiops: ability to match numbers and durations of human vocal bursts". — Science Vol 147: 300-301. [Crossref]
22. McGregor P.K., Krebs J.R., Perrins C.M. (1981). "Song repertoires and lifetime reproductive success in the great tit (Paris major)". — Am. Nat. Vol 118: 149-159. [Crossref]
23. Miller P.J.O., Shapiro A.D., Tyack P.L., Solow A.R. (2004). "Call-type matching in vocal exchanges of free-ranging resident killer whales, Orcinus orca". — Anim. Behav. Vol 67: 1099-1107. [Crossref]
24. Miller P.J.O. (2002). "Mixed-directionality of killer whale stereotyped calls: a direction of movement cue?" — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 52: 262-270. [Crossref]
25. Nemiroff L., Whitehead H. (2009). "Structural characteristcs of pulsed calls of long-finned pilot whale Globicephala melas". — Bioacoustics Vol 19: 67-92. [Crossref]
26. Nowicki S., Searcy W.A., Hughes M., Podos J. (2001). "The evolution of bird song: male and female response to song innovation in swamp sparrows". — Anim. Behav. Vol 62: 1189-1195. [Crossref]
27. Nowicki S., Searcy W.A. (2011). "Are better singers smarter?" — Behav. Ecol. Vol 22: 10-11. [Crossref]
28. Ottensmeyer C.A., Whitehead H. (2003). "Behavioural evidence for social units in long-finned pilot whales". — Can. J. Zool. Vol 81: 1327-1338. [Crossref]
29. Ramsey G., Bastian M.L., van Schaik C. (2007). "Animal innovation defined and operationalized". — Behav. Brain Sci. Vol 30: 393-437.
30. Rand S., Ryan M.J. (1981). "The adaptive significance of a complex vocal repetoire in a neotropical frog". — Z. Tierpsychol. Vol 57: 209-214. [Crossref]
31. Rankin S., Oswald J.N., Barlow J., Lammers M.O. (2007). "Patterned burst-pulse vocalizations of the northern right whale dolphin, Lissodelphis borealis". — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. Vol 121: 1213-1218. [Crossref]
32. Richards D.G., Wolz J.P., Herman L.M. (1984). "Vocal mimicry of computer-generated sounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 98: 10-28. [Crossref]
33. Ridgway S., Carder D., Jeffries M., Todd M. (2012). "Spontaneous human speech mimicry by a cetacean". — Curr. Biol. Vol 22: 860-861. [Crossref]
34. Sayigh L.S., Quick N., Hastie G., Tyack P. (2013). "Repeated call types in short-finned pilot whales, Globicephala macrorhynchus". — Mar. Mammal Sci. Vol 29: 312-324. [Crossref]
35. Schulz T.M., Whitehead H., Gero S., Rendell L. (2008). "Overlapping and matching of codas in vocal interactions between sperm whales: insights into communication function". — Anim. Behav. Vol 76: 1977-1988. [Crossref]
36. Sewall K.B., Soham J.A., Peters S., Nowicki S. (2013). "Potential trade-off between vocal ornamentation and spatial ability in a songbird". — Biol. Lett. Vol 9: 20130344. [Crossref]
37. Stirling I., Calvert W., Spencer C. (1987). "Evidence of stereotyped underwater vocalizations of male Atlantic walruses (Odobenus rosmarus rosmarus)". — Can. J. Zool. Vol 65: 2311-2321. [Crossref]
38. Strager H. (1995). "Pod-specific call repertoires and compound calls of killer whales, Orcinus orca Linnaeus, 1758, in the waters of northern Norway". — Can. J. Zool. Vol 73: 1037-1047. [Crossref]
39. Taruski A.G. (1979). "The whistle repetoire of the North Atlantic pilot whale (Globicephala melaena) and its relationship to behaviour and the environment". — In: Behav. mar. anim., Vol Vol. 3 ( Winn H.E., Olla B.L., eds). Plenum Press, New York, NY, p.  345-368. [Crossref]
40. Tyack P.L. (1986). "Whistle repertoires of two bottlenosed dolphins, Tursiops truncatus: mimicry of signature whistles?" — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 18: 251-257. [Crossref]
41. van Schaik C.P., Pradhan G.R. (2003). "A model for tool-use traditions in primates: implications for the coevolution of culture and cognition". — J. Hum. Evol. Vol 44: 645-664. [Crossref]
42. Vergara V., Barrett-Lennard L.G. (2008). "Vocal development in a beluga calf (Delphinapterus leucas)". — Aquat. Mamm. Vol 34: 123-143. [Crossref]
43. Wagner W.E. (1989). "Graded aggressive signals in Blanchard’s cricket frog: vocal responses to opponent proximity and size". — Anim. Behav. Vol 38: 1025-1038. [Crossref]
44. Weilgart L.S., Whitehead H. (1990). "Vocalizations of the North Atlantic pilot whale (Globicephala melas) as related to behavioral contexts". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 26: 399-402. [Crossref]
45. Weinrich M.T., Schilling M.R., Belt C.R. (1992). "Evidence for aquisition of a novel feeding behaviour: lobtail feeding in humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae". — Anim. Behav. Vol 44: 1059-1072. [Crossref]
46. Whitehead H., Rendell L., Osborne R.W., Würsig B. (2004). "Culture and conservation of non-humans with reference to whales and dolphins: review and new directions". — Biol. Conserv. Vol 120: 431-441. [Crossref]
47. Whitehead H., Weilgart L.S. (1991). "Patterns of visually observable behaviour and vocalizations in groups of female sperm whales". — Behaviour Vol 118: 275-296. [Crossref]
48. Zwamborn E.M.J., Whitehead H. (2017). "Repeated call sequences and behavioural context in long-finned pilot whales off Cape Breton, Nova Scotia, Canada". — Bioacoustics Vol 26: 169-183. DOI:10.1080/09524622.2016.1233457. [Crossref]
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003451
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003451
2017-10-23
2018-02-23

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation