Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Grooming interchange for resource tolerance: biological markets principles within a group of free-ranging rhesus macaques

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Behaviour

In group-living animals, allogrooming is a common, heterogeneously distributed affiliative behaviour. Among non-human primates, Barrett et al. (1999) predicted ways in which Biological Markets principles interact with competitive regimes to influence grooming reciprocity and interchange. Most tests of these predictions, done at a group level, have produced inconsistent results. Here we take a novel approach by testing these predictions across individuals within a group. This is based on the premise that in groups facing moderate-to-high within-group-competition, individuals vary in their abilities to access resources based on their competitive abilities, causing them to pursue different grooming exchange strategies. We examine evidence for grooming reciprocity and interchange for tolerance at drinking sources among adult females within a group of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) on Cayo Santiago. We test the above premise by assessing hierarchical steepness, and the relationship between individuals’ David’s scores (DS) and access to drinking sources. Finally, we examine the relationship of DS with grooming reciprocity and interchange to see whether they are consistent with the operation of market forces among individuals. Social network comparisons revealed that giving grooming was strongly predicted by both receiving drinking tolerance (interchange) and receiving grooming (reciprocity), despite strong associations with proximity and maternal kinship. The group showed a moderately steep hierarchy, and negative correlations between individuals’ David’s scores and difficulties in accessing drinking stations. Finally, we found partial support for a market-based explanation. Individuals with relatively low David’s scores were more likely to interchange grooming with drinking tolerance. However, grooming reciprocity wasn’t greater among individuals with higher David’s scores. Our findings suggest that multiple explanatory frameworks — reciprocity, market-based interchange, and/or proximity-mediated interchange/social bond investment — may all shape rhesus grooming exchange patterns. Future directions include examining evidence for additional forms of grooming interchange, and the influence of between-group-competition and stress-indicators on grooming reciprocity.

Affiliations: 1: aDepartment of Population Health & Reproduction, School of Veterinary Medicine, UC Davis, Davis, CA, USA ; 2: bDepartment of Anthropology and Graduate Program in Evolution, Ecology and Behavior, University at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY, USA

*Corresponding author’s e-mail address: krishnanatarajan@ucdavis.edu
10.1163/1568539X-00003462
/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003462
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
10
5
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003462
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Altmann J. (1974). "Observational study of behavior: sampling methods". — Behaviour Vol 49: 227-266. [Crossref]
2. Aureli F., Preston S.D., de Waal F.B.M. (1999). "Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): a pilot study". — J. Comp. Psychol. Vol 113: 59-65. [Crossref]
3. Aureli F., Schaffner C.M. (2002). "Relationship assessment through emotional mediation". — Behaviour Vol 139: 393-420. [Crossref]
4. Balasubramaniam K.N., Berman C.M., Ogawa H., Li J. (2011). "Using biological markets principles to examine patterns of grooming exchange in Macaca thibetana". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 73: 1269-1279. DOI:10.1002/ajp.20999. [Crossref]
5. Balasubramaniam K.N., Dittmar K., Berman C.M., Butovskaya M., Cooper M.A., Majolo B., de Waal F.B.M. (2012). "Hierarchical steepness and phylogenetic models: phylogenetic signals in Macaca". — Anim. Behav. Vol 83: 1207-1218. [Crossref]
6. Balasubramaniam K.N., Berman C.M., De Marco A., Dittmar K., Majolo B., Ogawa H., de Vries H. (2013). "Consistency of dominance rank order: a comparison of David’s scores with I&SI and Bayesian methods in macaques". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 75: 959-971. [Crossref]
7. Balasubramaniam K.N., Dunayer E.S., Gilhooly L.J., Rosenfield K.A., Berman C.M. (2014). "Group size, contest competition, and social structure in Cayo Santiago rhesus macaques". — Behaviour Vol 151: 1759-1798. [Crossref]
8. Balasubramaniam K.N., Beisner B.A., Vandeleest J., Atwill E.R., McCowan B. (2016). "Social buffering and contact transmission: network connections have beneficial and detrimental effects on shigella infection risk among captive rhesus macaques". — PeerJ Vol 4: e2630. DOI:10.7717/peerj.2630. [Crossref]
9. Barrett L., Gaynor D., Henzi S.P. (2002). "A dynamic interaction between aggression and grooming reciprocity among female chacma baboons". — Anim. Behav. Vol 63: 1047-1053. [Crossref]
10. Barrett L., Henzi S.P. (2002). "Constraints on relationship formation among female primates". — Behaviour Vol 139: 263-289. [Crossref]
11. Barrett L., Henzi S.P., Weingrill T., Lycett J.E., Hill R.A. (1999). "Market forces predict grooming reciprocity in female baboons". — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 266: 665-670. [Crossref]
12. Barton R.A. (1987). "Allogrooming as mutualism in diurnal lemurs". — Primates Vol 28: 539-542. [Crossref]
13. Bates D., Maechler M., Bolker B., Walker S., Christensen R.H.B., Singmann H., Green P. (2016). Linear mixed-effects models using ‘eigen’ and S4. Available online at https://github.com/lme4/lme4/, http://lme4.r-forge.r-project.org/.
14. Berman C.M. (2011). "Kinship: family ties and social behavior". — In: Primates in perspective, 2nd edn. ( Campbell C.J., Fuentes A., MacKinnon K.C., Panger M., Bearder S.K., eds). Oxford University Press, New York, NY, p.  576-587.
15. Berman C.M., Kapsalis E. (2009). "Variation over time in grooming kin bias among female rhesus macaques on Cayo Santiago supports the time constraints hypothesis". — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. Vol 48: 89-90.
16. Berman C.M., Thierry B. (2010). "Variation in kin bias: species differences and time constraints in macaques". — Behaviour Vol 147: 1863-1887. DOI:10.1163/000579510X539691. [Crossref]
17. Boelkins R.C., Wilson A.P. (1972). "Intergroup social dynamics of the Cayo Santiago rhesus (Macaca mulatta) with special reference to changes in group membership by males". — Primates Vol 13: 125-139. [Crossref]
18. Brent L.J.N., Semple S., Dubuc C., Heistermann M., MacLarnon A. (2010). "Social capital and physiological stress levels in adult female rhesus macaques". — Physiol. Behav. Vol 102: 76-83. [Crossref]
19. Brosnan S.F., de Waal F.B.M. (2002). "A proximate perspective on reciprocal altruism". — Hum. Nat. Vol 13: 129-152. [Crossref]
20. Burnham K.P., Anderson D.R., Huyvaert K.P. (2011). "AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: some background, observations, and comparisons". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 65: 23-35. [Crossref]
21. Butts C.T. (2008). "Social network analysis with sna". — J. Stat. Softw. Vol 24: 1-51.
22. Carne C., Viper S., Semple S. (2011). "Reciprocation and interchange of grooming, agonistic support, feeding tolerance, and aggression in semi-free-ranging Barbary macaques". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 73: 1127-1133. [Crossref]
23. Carter G., Leffer L. (2015). "Social grooming in bats: are vampire bats exceptional?" — PLoS ONE Vol 10: e0138430.
24. Carter G., Farine D.R., Wilkinson G.S. (2017). "Social bet-hedging in vampire bats". — Biol. Lett. Vol 13: 20170112. [Crossref]
25. Chapais B. (2006). "Kinship, competence and cooperation in primates". — In: Cooperation in primates and humans: an evolutionary perspective ( Kappeler P.M., van Schaik C.P., eds). Springer, Berlin, p.  47-64. [Crossref]
26. Clutton-Brock T. (2002). "Breeding together: cooperation between non-kin in animal societies". — Science Vol 296: 69-72. [Crossref]
27. Cords M. (1995). "Predator vigilance costs of allogrooming in wild blue monkeys". — Behaviour Vol 132: 559-569. [Crossref]
28. Cords M. (2013). "The behavior, ecology, and social evolution of cercopithecine monkeys". — In: The evolution of primate societies ( Mitani J.C., Call J., Kappeler P.M., Palombit R.A., Silk J.B., eds). The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, p.  91-112.
29. de Vries H. (1998). "Finding a dominance order most consistent with a linear hierarchy: a new procedure and review". — Anim. Behav. Vol 55: 827-843. [Crossref]
30. de Vries H., Netto W.J., Hanegraaf P.L.H. (1993). "Matman: a program for the analysis of sociometric matrices and behavioral transition matrices". — Behaviour Vol 125: 157-175. [Crossref]
31. de Vries H., Stevens J.M.G., Vervaecke H. (2006). "Measuring and testing the steepness of dominance hierarchies". — Anim. Behav. Vol 71: 585-592. [Crossref]
32. Dekker D., Krackhardt D., Snijders T.A.B. (2007). "Sensitivity of MR-QAP tests to collinearity and autocorrelation conditions". — Psychometrika Vol 72: 563-581. [Crossref]
33. Dubuc C., Hughes K.D., Cascio J., Santos L.R. (2012). "Social tolerance in a despotic primate: co-feeding between consortship partners in rhesus macaques". — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. Vol 148: 73-80. [Crossref]
34. Dunayer E.S., Berman C.M. (2016). "Biological markets: theory, interpretation, and proximate perspectives. A response to Sanchez-Amaro and Amici (2015)". — Anim. Behav. Vol 121: 131-136. [Crossref]
35. Dunbar R.I. (1992). "Time: a hidden constraint on the behavioral ecology of baboons". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 31: 35-49. [Crossref]
36. Fischer M.K., Hoffmann K.H., Völkl W. (2001). "Competition for mutualists in an ant-homopteran interaction mediated by hierarchies of ant attendance". — Oikos Vol 92: 531-541. [Crossref]
37. Fox J., Weisberg S. (2011). An R companion to applied regression, 2nd edn. — Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
38. Fruteau C., van de Waal E., Van Damme E., Noë R. (2011). "Infant access and handling in sooty mangabeys and vervet monkeys". — Anim. Behav. Vol 81: 153-161. [Crossref]
39. Gammell M.P., de Vries H., Jennings D.J., Carlin C.M., Hayden T.J. (2003). "David’s score: a more appropriate dominance ranking method than Clutton-Brock et al.’s index". — Anim. Behav. Vol 66: 601-605. [Crossref]
40. Gelman A., Su Y.S., Yajima M., Hill J., Pittau M.G., Kerm J. (2009). arm: data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. R package, version 9.01. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna.
41. Grueber C.E., Nakagawa S., Laws R.J., Jamieson I.G. (2011). "Multimodel inference in ecology and evolution: challenges and solutions". — J. Evol. Biol. Vol 24: 699-711. [Crossref]
42. Gumert M.D. (2007). "Grooming and infant handling interchange in Macaca fascicularis: the relationship between infant supply and grooming payment". — Int. J. Primatol. Vol 28: 1059-1074. [Crossref]
43. Gumert M.D. (2011). "A common monkey of southeast Asia: longtailed macaque populations, ethnophoresy, and their occurrence in human environments". — In: Monkeys on the edge: ecology and management of longtailed macaques and their interface with humans ( Gumert M.D., Fuentes A., Jones-Engel L., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  3-43. [Crossref]
44. Hamilton W.J. (1964). "The genetical evolution of social behaviour, I, II". — J. Theor. Biol. Vol 7: 1-52. [Crossref]
45. Hammerstein P., Noë R. (2016). "Biological trade and markets". — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 371: 20150101. [Crossref]
46. Handcock M., Hunter D., Butts C., Goodreau S., Morris M. (2006). Statnet: An R package for the statistical analysis and simulation of social networks. Available online at http://www.csde.washington.edu/statnet.
47. Hanneman R.A., Riddle M. (2005). Introduction to social network methods. — University of California Riverside, Riverside, CA.
48. Hart B.L., Hart L. (1992). "Reciprocal allogrooming in impala". — Anim. Behav. Vol 44: 1073-1083. [Crossref]
49. Hausfater G. (1972). "Intergroup behavior of free-ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)". — Folia Primatol. Vol 18: 78-107. [Crossref]
50. Henzi S.P., Barrett L. (1999). "The value of grooming to female primates". — Primates Vol 40: 47-59. [Crossref]
51. Henzi S.P., Barrett L. (2002). "Infants as a commodity in a baboon market". — Anim. Behav. Vol 63: 915-921. [Crossref]
52. Hinde R.A. (1976). "Interactions, relationships and social structure". — Man Vol 11: 1-17. [Crossref]
53. Hollenbeck A.R. (1978). "Problems of reliability in observational research". — In: Observing behaviour: data collection and analysis methods ( Sackett G.P., ed.). University Park Press, Baltimore, MD, p.  79-97.
54. Hubert L.J. (1987). Assignment methods in combinatorial data analysis. — Marcel Dekker, New York, NY.
55. Kaburu S.S.K., Newton-Fisher N.E. (2015). "Egalitarian despots: hierarchy steepness, reciprocity and the grooming-trade model in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes". — Anim. Behav. Vol 99: 61-71. [Crossref]
56. Kaburu S.S.K., Newton-Fisher N.E. (2016). "Markets misinterpreted? A comment on Sánchez-Amaro and Amici (2015)". — Anim. Behav. Vol 119: e1-e5. [Crossref]
57. Kappeler P.M., Van Schaik C.P. (2002). "Evolution of primate social systems". — Int. J. Primatol. Vol 23: 707-740. [Crossref]
58. Kappeler P.M., Cremer S., Nunn C.L. (2015). "Sociality and health: impacts of sociality on disease susceptibility and transmission in animal and human societies". — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 370: 20140116. [Crossref]
59. Kapsalis E., Berman C.M. (1996a). "Models of affiliative relationships among free-ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) I. Criteria for kinship". — Behaviour Vol 133: 1209-1234. [Crossref]
60. Kapsalis E., Berman C.M. (1996b). "Models of affiliative relationships among free-ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) II: testing predictions for three hypothesized organizing principles". — Behaviour Vol 133: 1235-1263. [Crossref]
61. Kessler M.J., Rawlins R.G. (2016). "A 75-year pictorial history of the Cayo Santiago rhesus monkey colony". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 78: 6-43. [Crossref]
62. Keverne E.B., Martensz N., Tuite B. (1989). "Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships". — Psychoneuroendocrinology Vol 14: 155-161. [Crossref]
63. Koenig A., Scarry C.J., Wheeler B.C., Borries C. (2013). "Variation in grouping patterns, mating systems and social structure: what socio-ecological models attempt to explain". — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. Vol 368: 20120348. [Crossref]
64. Krackhardt D. (1987). "QAP partialling as a test of spuriousness". — Soc. Netw. Vol 9: 171-186. [Crossref]
65. Leimar O., Axen A.H. (1993). "Strategic behaviour in an interspecific mutualism: interactions between lycaenid larvae and ants". — Anim. Behav. Vol 46: 1177-1182. [Crossref]
66. Maestripieri D., Hoffmann C.L. (2012). "Behavior and social dynamics of rhesus macaques at Cayo Santiago". — In: Bones, genetics, and behavior of rhesus macaques: Macaca mulatta of Cayo Santiago and beyond ( Wang Q., ed.). Springer, New York, NY, p.  247-262. [Crossref]
67. Majolo B., Vizioli D.B., Lehmann J. (2016). "The effect of intergroup competition on intragroup affiliation in primates". — Anim. Behav. Vol 114: 13-19. [Crossref]
68. Moore D., Angel J.E., Cheeseman I.M., Robinson G.E., Fahrbach S.E. (1995). "A highly specialized social grooming honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae)". — J. Insect Behav. Vol 8: 855-861. [Crossref]
69. Nakagawa S., Freckleton R.P. (2010). "Model averaging, missing data and multiple imputation: a case study for behavioural ecology". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 65: 103-116. [Crossref]
70. Noë R. (2001). "Biological markets: partner choice as the driving force behind the evolution of mutualisms". — In: Economics in nature: social dilemmas, mate choice, and biological markets ( Noë R., van Hooff J.A.R.A.M., Hammerstein P., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  93-118. [Crossref]
71. Noë R. (2016). How do biological markets compare to the markets of economics? — MPRA Paper No. 72509.
72. Noë R., Hammerstein P. (1994). "Biological markets: supply and demand determine the effects of partner choice in cooperation, mutualism and mating". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 35: 1-11. [Crossref]
73. Noë R., Hammerstein P. (1995). "Biological markets". — Trends Ecol. Evol. Vol 10: 336-340. [Crossref]
74. Nunn C.L. (2012). "Primate disease ecology in comparative and theoretical perspective". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 74: 497-509. DOI:10.1002/ajp.21986. [Crossref]
75. Radhakrishna S., Huffman M.A., Sinha A. (eds) (2013). The macaque connection. — Springer, New York, NY.
76. Rawlins R.G., Kessler M.J. (1986). The Cayo Santiago macaques: history, behavior, and biology. — SUNY Press, Albany, NY.
77. Sanchez-Amaro A., Amici F. (2015). "Are primates out of the market?" — Anim. Behav. Vol 110: 51-60. [Crossref]
78. Schino G. (2001). "Grooming, competition and social rank among female primates: a meta-analysis". — Anim. Behav. Vol 62: 265-271. [Crossref]
79. Schino G. (2007). "Grooming and agonistic support: a metaanalysis of primate reciprocal altruism". — Behav. Ecol. Vol 18: 115-120. [Crossref]
80. Schino G., Aureli F. (2008). "Tradeoffs in primate grooming reciprocation: testing behavioral flexibility and correlated evolution". — Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Vol 95: 439-446. [Crossref]
81. Schino G., Aureli F. (2009). "Reciprocal altruism in primates: partner choice, cognition, and emotions". — Adv. Stud. Behav. Vol 39: 45-69. [Crossref]
82. Seyfarth R.M. (1977). "A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys". — J. Theor. Biol. Vol 65: 671-698. [Crossref]
83. Seyfarth R.M., Cheney D.L. (1984). "Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys". — Nature Vol 308: 541-543. [Crossref]
84. Seyfarth R.M., Cheney D.L. (2012). "The evolutionary origins of friendship". — Annu. Rev. Psychol. Vol 63: 153-177. [Crossref]
85. Silk J.B., Alberts S.C., Altmann J. (2003). "Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival". — Science Vol 302: 1231-1234. [Crossref]
86. Silk J.B., Alberts S.C., Altmann J. (2004). "Patterns of coalition formation by adult female baboons in Amboseli, Kenya". — Anim. Behav. Vol 67: 573-582. [Crossref]
87. Silk J.B., Seyfarth R.M., Cheney D.L. (1999). "The structure of social relationships among female savannah baboons in Moremi Reserve, Botswana". — Behaviour Vol 136: 679-703. [Crossref]
88. Silk J.B., Seyfarth R.M., Cheney D.L. (2013). "A practical guide to the study of social relationships". — Evol. Anthropol. Vol 22: 213-225. [Crossref]
89. Smutt K., MacLarnon A., Heistermann M., Semple S. (2007). "Grooming in Barbary macaques: better to give than to receive?" — Biol. Lett. Vol 3: 231-233. [Crossref]
90. Soares M.C., Côté I.M., Cardoso S.C., Oliveira R.F., Bshary R. (2010). "Caribbean cleaning gobies prefer client ectoparasites over mucus". — Ethology Vol 116: 1244-1248. [Crossref]
91. Southwick C.H., Siddiqi F. (2011). "India’s rhesus population: protection versus conservation management". — In: Monkeys on the edge: ecology and management of long-tailed macaques and their interface with humans ( Gumert M.D., Fuentes A., Jones-Engel L., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  275-292. [Crossref]
92. Sterck E.H.M., Watts D.P., van Schaik C.P. (1997). "The evolution of female social relationships in non-human primates". — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. Vol 41: 291-309. [Crossref]
93. Stevens J.M.G., Vervaecke H., de Vries H., van Elsacker L. (2005). "The influence of the steepness of dominance hierarchies on reciprocity and interchange in captive groups of bonobos (Pan paniscus)". — Anim. Behav. Vol 142: 941-960.
94. Thierry B. (2007). "Unity in diversity: lessons from macaque societies". — Evol. Anthropol. Vol 16: 224-238. DOI:10.1002/evan.20147. [Crossref]
95. Thierry B. (2013). "The macaques: a double-layered social organization". — In: Primates in perspective ( Campbell C.J., Fuentes A., MacKinnon K.C., Bearder S.K., Stumpf R.M., eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p.  229-240.
96. Tiddi B., Aureli F., Schino G. (2010). "Grooming for infant handling in tufted capuchin monkeys: a reappraisal of the primate infant market". — Anim. Behav. Vol 79: 1115-1123. [Crossref]
97. Tiddi B., Aureli F., Schino G. (2013). "Grooming up the hierarchy: the exchange of grooming and rank-related benefits in a new world primate". — PLoS ONE Vol 7: e36641. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0036641.
98. Trivers R.L. (1971). "The evolution of reciprocal altruism". — Q. Rev. Biol. Vol 46: 35-57. DOI:10.1086/406755. [Crossref]
99. VanderWaal K.L., Atwill E.R., Isbell L.A., McCowan B. (2014a). "Quantifying microbe transmission networks for wild and domestic ungulates in Kenya". — Biol. Conserv. Vol 169: 136-146. DOI:10.1016/j.biocon.2013.11.008. [Crossref]
100. Ventura R., Majolo B., Koyama N.F., Hardie S.M., Schino G. (2006). "Reciprocation and interchange in wild Japanese macaques: grooming, cofeeding and agonistic support". — Am. J. Primatol. Vol 68: 1138-1149. [Crossref]
101. Werner G.D.A., Kiers E.T. (2015). "Partner selection in the mycorrhizal mutualism". — New Phytol. Vol 205: 1437-1442. [Crossref]
102. Werner G.D.A., Strassmann J.E., Ivens A.B.F., Engelmoer D.J.P., Verbruggen E., Queller D.C., Kiers E.T. (2014). "Evolution of microbial markets". — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol 111: 1237-1244. [Crossref]
103. Whittingham M.J., Stephens P.A., Bradbury R.B., Freckleton R.P. (2006). "Why do we still use stepwise modelling in ecology and behaviour?" — J. Anim. Ecol. Vol 75: 1182-1189. [Crossref]
104. Wikberg E.C., Ting N., Sicotte P. (2014). "Kinship and similarity in residency status structure female social networks in black-and-white colobus monkeys (Colobus vellerosus)". — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. Vol 153: 365-376. [Crossref]
105. Young C., Majolo B., Heistermann M., Schulke O., Ostner J. (2014). "Responses to social and environmental stress are attenuated by strong male bonds in wild macaques". — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol 111: 18195-18200. [Crossref]
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003462
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/1568539x-00003462
2017-11-17
2018-09-24

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Behaviour — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation