Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Mission: Impossible? Modelling the Verbal Estimation of Duration

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Timing & Time Perception

Three participants produced a large number of verbal estimates of tone durations in the range of 77–1183 ms. Data from this task were simulated by an ‘attractor model’, which used the idea of competition between ‘attractors’ (‘quantized’ values output as verbal estimates) which differed in weight, and distance from the stimulus duration to be estimated. To produce an estimate, all attractors competed for priority as output values, with the final value being decided probabilistically. The model embodied underlying scalar representations of time, in the form of mean accuracy and constant coefficient of variation. The model was able to reconcile such scalar properties of time with deviations from scalar properties often found in verbal estimation data, such as declining coefficients of variation with increasing duration value. The model furthermore showed that multiplicative and additive changes in underlying time representations should be translated veridically into behaviour, although the attractor competition process could distort patterns and absolute values of underlying variance.

Affiliations: 1: Keele University, School of Psychology, Dorothy Hodgkin Building, Keele, Staffordshire ST5 5BG, UK

10.1163/22134468-03002051
/content/journals/10.1163/22134468-03002051
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
10
5
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134468-03002051
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Allan L. G. (1998). "The influence of the scalar timing model on human timing research". Behav. Process .,Vol 44, 101117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(98)00043-6
2. Allman M. J. ,, Teki S. , Griffiths T. D. ,, & Meck W. H. (2014). "Properties of the internal clock: First- and second-order principles of subjective time". Annu. Rev. Psychol ., Vol 65, 743771. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115117
3. Fraisse P. (1964). The psychology of time . London, UK: Eyre and Spottiswode.
4. Gibbon J. ,, Church R. M. , & Meck W. H. (1984). "Scalar timing in memory. Ann. N.Y ". Acad. Sci ., Vol 423, 5277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x
5. Gil S. ,, & Droit-Volet S. (2011). ‘Time flies in the presence of angry faces.’ depending on the temporal task used! Acta Psychol ., Vol 136, 354362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.12.010
6. Grondin S. ,, Meilleur-Wells G. , & Lachance R. (1999). "When to start explicit counting in a time-interval discrimination task". J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform .,Vol 25, 9931004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.25.4.993
7. Lejeune H. ,, & Wearden J. H. (2006). "Scalar properties in animal timing: Conformity and violations". Q. J. Exp. Psychol .,Vol 59,18751908. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210600784649
8. Matthews W. J. ,, & Meck W. H. (2014). "Time perception: The bad news and the good". Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci ., Vol 5, 429446. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1298
9. Meck W. H. (1983). "Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes". J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process ., Vol 9, 171201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.9.2.171
10. Meck W. H. (1996). "Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception". Cogn. Brain Res ., Vol 3, 227242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0926-6410(96)00009-2
11. Ogden R. ,, Moore D. ,, Redfern L. ,, & McGlone F. (2015). "The effect of pain and the anticipation of pain on temporal perception: A role for attention and arousal". Cogn. Emot .,Vol 29, 910922. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.954529
12. Penney T. B. ,, Gibbon J. ,, & Meck W. H. (2000). "Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory". J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform .,Vol 26, 17701787. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.26.6.1770
13. Penton-Voak I. P. ,, Edwards H. ,, Percival A. ,, & Wearden J. H. (1996). "Speeding up an internal clock in humans? Effects of click trains on subjective duration". J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process .,Vol 22, 307320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.22.3.307
14. Repp B. H. ,, Mendlowitz H. B. ,, & Hove M. J. (2013). "Does rapid auditory stimulation accelerate an internal pacemaker?" Don’t bet on it. Timing Time Percept .,Vol 1, 6576. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134468-00002001
15. Wearden J. H. (1992). "Temporal generalization in humans". J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process .,Vol 18, 134144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.18.2.134
16. Wearden J. H. , (2003). "Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges". In Helfrich H. (Ed.), Time and mind II: Information-processing perspectives (pp. 2139). Gottingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.
17. Wearden J. H. (2004). "Decision processes in models of timing". Acta Neurobiol. Exp ., Vol 64, 303317.
18. Wearden J. H. ,, & Bray S. (2001). "Scalar timing without reference memory: Episodic temporal generalization and bisection in humans". Q. J. Exp. Psychol .,Vol 54B,289310.
19. Wearden J. H. ,, & Ferrara A. (1995). "Stimulus spacing effects in temporal bisection by humans". Q. J. Exp. Psychol .,Vol 48B, 289310.
20. Wearden J. H. ,, & Ferrara A. (1996). "Stimulus range effects in temporal bisection by humans". Q. J. Exp. Psychol ., Vol 49B, 2444.
21. Wearden J. H. ,, & Lejeune H. (2008). "Scalar properties in human timing: Conformity and violations". Q. J. Exp. Psychol .,Vol 61,569587. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210701282576
22. Wearden J. H. ,, Edwards H. ,, Fakhri M. , & Percival A. (1998). "Why ‘sounds are judged longer than lights’: Application of a model of the internal clock in humans". Q. J. Exp. Psychol .,Vol 51B, 97120.
23. Wearden J. H. ,, Todd N. P. M. ,, & Jones L. A. (2006). "When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur?" Q. J. Exp. Psychol .,Vol 59,17091724. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210500314729
24. Wearden J. H. ,, Norton R. ,, Martin S. ,, & Montford-Bebb O. (2007). "Internal clock processes and the filled duration illusion". J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform .,Vol 33,716729. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.3.716
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/22134468-03002051
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134468-03002051
2015-12-10
2017-11-17

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Timing & Time Perception — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation