Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Using Published Trial Schematics to Assess a Brief (Spatial) History of Time: Questioning the Graphical Depiction of Experimental Procedures

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Timing & Time Perception

Trial schematics are ubiquitous within psychology journals articles and have the potential to inform how we think about time in space from a non-linguistic point of view. Graphical representations of trial schematics were used to compare the spatial representations of time used by the scientific community with the dominant spatial stereotypes for temporal events reported by the scientific community. From 294 observations, approximately 81% of trial schematics contained left-to-right and / or top-to-bottom representations of first-to-last events, consistent with the dominant Western spatial expressions of time. An initially counter-intuitive left-to-right but bottom-to-top spatial stereotype used in approximately 18% of schematics is discussed with respect to its potential perceptual origins. The complications that arise from the use of multiple spatial axes in the representation of time are highlighted and given the tendency for trial schematics to be informationally poor, alternative routes for the supply of thorough experimental detail are suggested.

Affiliations: 1: Department of Psychology, School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, BN1 9QH, UK

10.1163/22134468-03002054
/content/journals/10.1163/22134468-03002054
dcterms_title,pub_keyword,dcterms_description,pub_author
10
5
Loading
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134468-03002054
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. American Psychological Association (2006). The Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, D.C., USA: American Psychological Association.
2. Bergen B. K. , & Chan Lau T. T. (2012). "Writing direction affects how people map space onto time". Front Psychol , Vol 3: 109. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00109.
3. Boroditsky L. (2000). "Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors". Cognition , Vol 75, 128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6
4. Boroditsky L. (2001). "Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time". Cogn. Psychol ., Vol 43, 122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0748
5. Casasanto D. ,, & Boroditsky L. (2008). "Time in the mind: Using space to think about time". Cognition , Vol 106, 579593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004
6. Chan Z. ,, & Dyson B. J. (2015). "The effects of association strength and cross-modal correspondence on the development of multimodal stimuli". Atten. Percept. Psychophys , Vol 77, 560570. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0794-0
7. Chen J. (2007). "Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001) ". Cognition , Vol 104, 427436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.012
8. Gallo D. A. ,, Chen J. M. ,, Wiseman A. L. ,, Schacter D. L. ,, & Budson A. E. (2007). "Retrieval monitoring and anosognosia in Alzheimer’s Disease". Neuropsychology , Vol 21, 559568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.21.5.559
9. Horowitz T. S. ,, Fine E. M. ,, Fencsik D. E. ,, Yurgenson S. ,, & Wolfe J. M. (2007). "Fixational eye movements are not an index of covert attention". Psychol. Sci ., Vol 18, 356363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01903.x
10. Ishihara M. ,, Keller P. E. ,, Rossetti Y. ,, & Prinz W. (2008). "Horizontal spatial representations of time: Evidence for the STEARC effect". Cortex , Vol 44, 454461. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.010
11. Knight M. ,, & Mather M. (2009). "Reconciling findings of emotion-inducing memory enhancement and impairment of preceding items". Emotion , Vol 9, 763781. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017281
12. Koivisto M. ,, & Revonsuo A. (2007). "How meaning shapes seeing". Psychol. Sci ., Vol 18, 845849. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01989.x
13. Konefal J. ,, Duncan R. ,, Meub W. ,, & Winfield D. (2006). "Time perspective as a potential tool for psychotherapeutic intervention". Psychol. Rep ., Vol 99, 111120.
14. Ono F. ,, & Kitazawa S. (2010). "The effect of perceived motion-in-depth on time perception". Cognition , Vol 115, 140146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.006
15. Miller J. (1982). "Divided attention : Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals". Cogn. Psychol ., Vol 14, 247279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(82)90010-X
16. Novick L. R. (2006). "Understanding spatial diagram structure: An analysis of hierarchies, matrices, and networks". Q. J. Exp. Psychol ., Vol 59, 18261856. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210500298997
17. Sheppard D. M. ,, Duncan J. ,, Shapiro K. L. ,, & Hillstrom A. P. (2002). "Objects and events in the attentional blink". Psychol. Sci ., Vol 13, 410415. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00473
18. Tsai C-C ,., & Brass M . (2007). "Does the human motor system simulate Pinocchio’s actions? Coacting with a human hand versus and wooden hand in a dyadic interaction". Psychol. Sci ., Vol 18, 10581062. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02025.x
19. Tversky B. , (2001). "Spatial schemas in depictions". In Gattis M. (Ed.) Spatial schemas and abstract thought (pp. 79111). Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press.
20. Tufte E. (2001). The Visual Display of Quantitative Information (2nd ed.). Cheshire, CT, USA: Graphics Press.
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/22134468-03002054
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134468-03002054
2015-12-10
2017-11-20

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Timing & Time Perception — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation