Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Visuotactile Temporal Recalibration Transfers Across Different Locations

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Multisensory Research
For more content, see Seeing and Perceiving and Spatial Vision.

Following prolonged exposure to audiovisual asynchrony, an observer’s point of subjective simultaneity (PSS) shifts in the direction of the leading modality. It has been debated whether other sensory pairings, such as vision and touch, lead to a similar temporal recalibration, and if so, whether the internal timing mechanism underlying lag visuotactile adaptation is centralised or distributed. To address these questions, we adapted observers to vision- and tactile-leading visuotactile asynchrony on either their left or right hand side in different blocks. In one test condition, participants performed a simultaneity judgment on the adapted side (unilateral) and in another they performed a simultaneity judgment on the non-adapted side (contralateral). In a third condition, participants adapted concurrently to equal and opposite asynchronies on each side and were tested randomly on either hand (bilateral opposed). Results from the first two conditions show that observers recalibrate to visuotactile asynchronies, and that the recalibration transfers to the non-adapted side. These findings suggest a centralised recalibration mechanism not linked to the adapted side and predict no recalibration for the bilateral opposed condition, assuming the adapted effects were equal on each side. This was confirmed in the group of participants that adapted to vision- and tactile-leading asynchrony on the right and left hand side, respectively. However, the other group (vision-leading on the left and tactile-leading on the right) did show a recalibration effect, suggesting a distributed mechanism. We discuss these findings in terms of a hybrid model that assumes the co-existence of a centralised and distributed timing mechanism.

Affiliations: 1: School of Psychology, University of Sydney, A19 Griffith Taylor, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia

Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134808-00002498
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Alais D. , Newell F. N. , Mamassian P. ( 2010). "Multisensory processing in review: from physiology to behaviour", Seeing Perceiving Vol 23, 338. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/187847510X488603
2. Bakeman R. ( 2005). "Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs", Behav. Res. Meth. Vol 37, 379384. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03192707
3. Brainard D. ( 1997). "The psychophysics toolbox", Spat. Vis. Vol 10, 433436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357
4. Buhusi C. V. , Meck W. H. ( 2005). "What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing", Nat. Rev. Neurosci. Vol 6, 755765. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1764
5. Di Luca M. , Machulla T. , Ernst M. ( 2009). "Recalibration of multisensory simultaneity: cross-modal transfer coincides with a change in perceptual latency", J. Vis. Vol 9, 116.
6. Eagleman D. M. ( 2008). "Human time perception and its illusions", Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. Vol 18, 131136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.002
7. Fujisaki W. , Shimojo S. , Kashino M. , Nishida S. ( 2004). "Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity", Nat. Neurosci. Vol 7, 773778. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1268
8. Hanson J. V. M. , Heron J. , Whitaker D. ( 2008). "Recalibration of perceived time across sensory modalities", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 185, 347352. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1282-3
9. Harrar V. , Harris L. R. ( 2005). "Simultaneity constancy: detecting events with touch and vision", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 166, 465473. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-2386-7
10. Harrar V. , Harris L. R. ( 2008). "The effect of exposure to asynchronous audio, visual, and tactile stimulus combinations on the perception of simultaneity", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 186, 517524. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1253-0
11. Heron J. , Roach N. W. , Whitaker D. , Hanson J. V. M. ( 2010). "Attention regulates the plasticity of multisensory timing", Eur. J. Neurosci. Vol 31, 17551762. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07194.x
12. Heron J. , Roach N. W. , Hanson J. V. M. , McGraw P. V. , Whitaker D. ( 2012). "Audiovisual time perception is spatially specific", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 218, 477485. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3038-3
13. Ikumi N. , Soto-Faraco S. ( 2014). "Selective attention modulates the direction of audio-visual temporal recalibration", PloS One Vol 9, e99311. DOI: . http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099311
14. Keetels M. , Vroomen J. ( 2007). "No effect of auditory–visual spatial disparity on temporal recalibration", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 182, 559565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1012-2
15. Keetels M. , Vroomen J. ( 2008). "Temporal recalibration to tactile–visual asynchronous stimuli", Neurosci. Lett. Vol 430, 130134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2007.10.044
16. King A. , Palmer A. ( 1985). "Integration of visual and auditory information in bimodal neurones in the guinea-pig superior colliculus", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 60, 492500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00236934
17. Lawrence M. A. ( 2014). ez: easy analysis and visualization of factorial experiments (version 4.2-2). Retrieved from .
18. Lewis P. A. , Miall R. C. ( 2003). "Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: evidence from neuroimaging", Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. Vol 13, 250255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00036-9
19. Mauk M. D. , Buonomano D. V. ( 2004). "The neural basis of temporal processing", Annu. Rev. Neurosci. Vol 27, 307340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144247
20. Miyazaki M. , Yamamoto S. , Uchida S. , Kitazawa S. ( 2006). "Bayesian calibration of simultaneity in tactile temporal order judgment", Nat. Neurosci. Vol 9, 875877. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1712
21. Navarra J. , García-Morera J. , Spence C. ( 2012). "Temporal adaptation to audiovisual asynchrony generalizes across different sound frequencies", Front. Psychol. Vol 3, 152. DOI: . http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00152
22. R Core Team( 2013). R: a Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from .
23. Repp B. H. , Su Y.-H. ( 2013). "Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of recent research (2006–2012)", Psychon. Bull. Rev. Vol 20, 403452. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2
24. Roseboom W. , Arnold D. H. ( 2011). "Twice upon a time: multiple concurrent temporal recalibrations of audiovisual speech", Psychol. Sci. Vol 22, 872877. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611413293
25. Roseboom W. , Kawabe T. , Nishida S. ( 2013). "Audio-visual temporal recalibration can be constrained by content cues regardless of spatial overlap", Front. Psychol. Vol 4, 189. DOI: . http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00189
26. RStudio( 2013). RStudio: integrated development environment for R (version 0.98.490), RStudio, Boston, MA, USA.
27. Stetson C. , Cui X. , Montague P. R. , Eagleman D. M. ( 2006). "Motor-sensory recalibration leads to an illusory reversal of action and sensation", Neuron Vol 51, 651659. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.006
28. Sugano Y. , Keetels M. , Vroomen J. ( 2014). "Concurrent sensorimotor temporal recalibration to different lags for the left and right hand", Front. Psychol. Vol 5, 140. DOI: . http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00140
29. Sugita Y. , Suzuki Y. ( 2003). "Implicit estimation of sound-arrival time", Nature Vol 421, 911912. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/421911a
30. Takahashi K. , Saiki J. , Watanabe K. ( 2008). "Realignment of temporal simultaneity between vision and touch", Neuroreport Vol 19, 319322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f4f039
31. Van der Burg E. , Alais D. , Cass J. ( 2013). "Rapid recalibration to audiovisual asynchrony", J. Neurosci. Vol 33, 1463314637. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1182-13.2013
32. Van der Burg E. , Orchard-Mills E. , Alais D. ( 2015). "Rapid temporal recalibration is unique to audiovisual stimuli", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 233, 5359. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4085-8
33. Vroomen J. , Keetels M. , de Gelder B. , Bertelson P. ( 2004). "Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony", Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. Vol 22, 3235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.07.003
34. Yarrow K. , Sverdrup-Stueland I. , Roseboom W. , Arnold D. H. ( 2013). "Sensorimotor temporal recalibration within and across limbs", J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. Vol 39, 16781689. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032534
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/22134808-00002498
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134808-00002498
2015-04-30
2018-07-16

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Multisensory Research — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation