Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Interactions Between Auditory Elevation, Auditory Pitch and Visual Elevation During Multisensory Perception

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this advance article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality
image of Multisensory Research

Affiliations: 1: 1Department of Neurology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA ; 2: 2Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA ; 3: 4Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Atlanta VAMC, Decatur, GA 30033, USA ; 4: 3Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA

*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: krish.sathian@emory.edu
Loading data from figshare Loading data from figshare
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134808-00002553
Loading

Data & Media loading...

http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/22134808-00002553
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134808-00002553
2017-04-04
2017-05-23
1. Algom D., Fitousi D. (2016). "Half a century of research on Garner interference and the separability-integrality distinction", Psychol. Bull. Vol 142, 13521383. [Crossref]
2. Audacity Team (2012). Audacity v2.0.1 [Computer program]. Retrieved from http://audacity.sourceforge.net/ Audacity ® software is copyright © 1999–2014 Audacity Team.
3. Barsalou L. W. (2008). "Grounded cognition", Annu. Rev. Psychol. Vol 59, 617645. [Crossref]
4. Batteau D. W. (1967). "The role of the pinna in human localization", Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. Vol 168, 158180. [Crossref]
5. Ben-Artzi E., Marks L. E. (1995). "Visual-auditory interaction in speeded classification: role of stimulus difference", Percept. Psychophys. Vol 57, 11511162. [Crossref]
6. Ben-Artzi E., Marks L. E. (1999). "Processing linguistic and perceptual dimensions of speech: interactions in speeded classification", J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. Vol 25, 579595. [Crossref]
7. Bernstein I. H., Edelstein B. A. (1971). "Effects of some variations in auditory input upon visual choice reaction time", J. Exp. Psychol. Vol 87, 241247. [Crossref]
8. Bertelson P., Aschersleben G. (1998). "Automatic bias of perceived auditory location", Psychon. Bull. Rev. Vol 5, 482489. [Crossref]
9. Carnevale M. J., Harris L. R. (2016). "Which direction is up for a high pitch?" Multisens. Res. Vol 29, 113132. [Crossref]
10. Chiou R., Rich A. N. (2012). "Cross-modality correspondence between pitch and spatial location modulates attentional orienting", Perception Vol 41, 339353. [Crossref]
11. Dolscheid S., Hunnius S., Casasanto D., Majid A. (2014). "Prelinguistic infants are sensitive to space-pitch associations found across cultures", Psychol. Sci. Vol 25, 12561261. [Crossref]
12. Evans K. K., Treisman A. (2010). "Natural cross-modal mappings between visual and auditory features", J. Vis. Vol 10(1) 6, 112. doi: .
13. Fitch W. T. (2000). "The evolution of speech: a comparative review", Trends Cogn. Sci. Vol 4, 258267. [Crossref]
14. Gallace A., Spence C. (2006). "Multisensory synesthetic interactions in the speeded classification of visual size", Percept. Psychophys. Vol 68, 11911203. [Crossref]
15. Garner W. R. (1974). The Processing of Information and Structure. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Potomac, MD, USA.
16. Köhler W. (1929). Gestalt Psychology. Liveright, New York, NY, USA.
17. Köhler W. (1947). Gestalt Psychology: an Introduction to New Concepts in Modern Psychology. Liveright, New York, NY, USA.
18. Lacey S., Martinez M. O., McCormick K., Sathian K. (2016). "Synesthesia strengthens sound-symbolic cross-modal correspondences", Eur. J. Neurosci. Vol 44, 27162721. [Crossref]
19. Oertel D., Doupe A. J. (2013). "The auditory central nervous system", in: Principles of Neural Science, 5th edn., Kandel E. R., Schwartz J. H., Jessell T. M., Siegelbaum S. A., Hudspeth A. J. (Eds), pp.  682711. McGraw Hill Medical, New York, NY, USA.
20. Parise C. V., Knorre K., Ernst M. O. (2014). "Natural auditory scene statistics shapes human spatial hearing", Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA Vol 111, 61046108. [Crossref]
21. Parkinson C., Kohler P. J., Sievers B., Wheatley T. (2012). "Associations between auditory pitch and visual elevation do not depend on language: evidence from a remote population", Perception Vol 41, 854861. [Crossref]
22. Pratt C. C. (1930). "The spatial character of high and low tones", J. Exp. Psychol. Vol 13, 278285. [Crossref]
23. Roffler S. K., Butler R. A. (1968). "Factors that influence the localization of sound in the vertical plane", J. Acoust. Soc. Am. Vol 43, 12551259. [Crossref]
24. Rusconi E., Kwan B., Giordano B. L., Umilta C., Butterworth B. (2006). "Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect", Cognition Vol 99, 113129. [Crossref]
25. Shor R. E. (1975). "An auditory analog of the Stroop test", J. Gen. Psychol. Vol 93, 281288.
26. Spence C. (2011). "Crossmodal correspondences: a tutorial review", Atten. Percept. Psychol. Vol 73, 971995. [Crossref]
27. Spence C. (2013). "Just how important is spatial coincidence to multisensory integration? Evaluating the spatial rule", Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. Vol 1296, 3149. [Crossref]
28. Stein B. E. (1998). "Neural mechanisms for synthesizing sensory information and producing adaptive behaviors", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 123, 124135. [Crossref]
29. Walker L., Walker P., Francis B. (2012). "A common scheme for cross-sensory correspondences across stimulus domains", Perception Vol 41, 11861192. [Crossref]
30. Walker P., Bremner J. G., Mason U., Spring J., Mattock K., Slater A., Johnson S. P. (2010). "Preverbal infants’ sensitivity to synaesthetic cross-modality correspondences", Psychol. Sci. Vol 21, 2125. [Crossref]
31. Wallace M. T., Roberson G. E., Hairston W. D., Stein B. E., Vaughan J. W., Schirillo J. A. (2004). "Unifying multisensory signals across time and space", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 158, 252258. [Crossref]
32. Ward L. M. (1994). "Supramodal and modality-specific mechanisms for stimulus-driven shifts of auditory and visual attention", Can. J. Exp. Psychol. Vol 48, 242259. [Crossref]
Submit comment
Close
Comment moderation successfully completed

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation