Cookies Policy
X

This site uses cookies. By continuing to browse the site you are agreeing to our use of cookies.

I accept this policy

Find out more here

Auditory Rhythms Influence Judged Time to Contact of an Occluded Moving Object

No metrics data to plot.
The attempt to load metrics for this article has failed.
The attempt to plot a graph for these metrics has failed.
The full text of this article is not currently available.

Brill’s MyBook program is exclusively available on BrillOnline Books and Journals. Students and scholars affiliated with an institution that has purchased a Brill E-Book on the BrillOnline platform automatically have access to the MyBook option for the title(s) acquired by the Library. Brill MyBook is a print-on-demand paperback copy which is sold at a favorably uniform low price.

Access this article

+ Tax (if applicable)
Add to Favorites
You must be logged in to use this functionality

image of Multisensory Research
For more content, see Seeing and Perceiving and Spatial Vision.

We studied the expected moment of reappearance of a moving object after it disappeared from sight. In particular, we investigated whether auditory rhythms influence time to contact (TTC) judgments. Using displays in which a moving disk disappears behind an occluder, we examined whether an accompanying auditory rhythm influences the expected TTC of an occluded moving object. We manipulated a baseline auditory rhythm — consisting of equal sound and pause durations — in two ways: either the pause durations or the sound durations were increased to create slower rhythms. Participants had to press a button at the moment they expected the disk to reappear. Variations in pause duration (Experiments 1 and 2) affected expected TTC, in contrast to variations in sound duration (Experiment 3). These results show that auditory rhythms affect expected reappearance of an occluded moving object. Second, these results suggest that temporal auditory grouping is an important factor in TTC.

Affiliations: 1: Radboud University, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: c.chotsrisuparat@donders.ru.nl
Loading data from figshare Loading data from figshare
Loading

Full text loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134808-00002592
Loading

Data & Media loading...

1. Battaglia P. W., Jacobs R. A., Aslin R. N. (2003). "Bayesian integration of visual and auditory signals for spatial localization", J. Opt. Soc. Am. A Vol 20, 13911397. [Crossref]
2. Baurès R., Bennett S. J., Causer J. (2015). "Temporal estimation with two moving objects: overt and covert pursuit", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 233, 253261. [Crossref]
3. Burke L. (1952). "On the tunnel effect", Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Vol 4, 121138. [Crossref]
4. Burr D., Banks M. S., Morrone M. C. (2009). "Auditory dominance over vision in the perception of interval duration", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 198, 4957. [Crossref]
5. Chien S. E., Ono F., Watanabe K. (2013). "A transient auditory signal shifts the perceived offset position of a moving visual object", Front. Psychol. Vol 4, 70. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00070. [Crossref]
6. DeLucia P. R., Kaiser M. K., Bush J. M., Meyer L. E., Sweet B. T. (2003). "Information integration in judgments about time to contact", Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Vol 56, 11651189. [Crossref]
7. DeLucia P. R., Preddy D., Oberfeld D. (2016). "Audiovisual integration of time-to-contact information for approaching objects", Multisens. Res. Vol 29, 365395. [Crossref]
8. Flombaum J. I., Scholl B. J. (2006). "A temporal same-object advantage in the tunnel effect: facilitated change-detection for persisting objects", J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. Vol 32, 840853. [Crossref]
9. Flombaum J. I., Scholl B. J., Pylyshyn Z. W. (2008). "Attentional resources in visual tracking through occlusion: the high-beams effect", Cognition Vol 107, 904931. [Crossref]
10. Foley A. J., Michaluk L. M., Thomas D. G. (2004). "Pace alteration and estimation of time intervals", Percept. Mot. Skills Vol 98, 291298. [Crossref]
11. Geiser E., Gabrieli J. D. (2013). "Influence of rhythmic grouping on duration perception: a novel auditory illusion", PLoS One Vol 8, e54273. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0054273.
12. Getzmann S. (2007). "The effect of brief auditory stimuli on visual apparent motion", Perception Vol 36, 10891103. [Crossref]
13. Gordon M. S., Rosenblum L. D. (2005). "Effects of intrastimulus modality change on audiovisual time-to-arrival judgments", Percept. Psychophys. Vol 67, 580594. [Crossref]
14. Hanson J. V. M., Heron J., Whitaker D. (2008). "Recalibration of perceived time across sensory modalities", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 185, 347352. [Crossref]
15. Harrison N. R., Wuerger S. M., Meyer G. F. (2010). "Reaction time facilitation for horizontally moving auditory–visual stimuli", J. Vis. Vol 10, 16. DOI:10.1167/10.14.16.
16. Hecht H., Savelsbergh G. J. (Eds) (2004). Time-to-Contact. Elsevier, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
17. Hidaka S., Teramoto W., Gyoba J., Suzuki Y. (2010). "Sound can prolong the visible persistence of moving visual objects", Vis. Res. Vol 50, 20932099. [Crossref]
18. Hidaka S., Teramoto W., Sugita Y. (2015). "Spatiotemporal processing in crossmodal interactions for perception of the external world: a review", Front. Integr. Neurosci. Vol 9. DOI:10.3389/fnint.2015.00062. [Crossref]
19. Hofbauer M., Wuerger S. M., Meyer G. F., Roehrbein F., Schill K., Zetzsche C. (2004). "Catching audiovisual mice: predicting the arrival time of auditory-visual motion signals", Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. Vol 4, 241250. [Crossref]
20. Horr N. K., Di Luca M. (2015). "Timing rhythms: perceived duration increases with a predictable temporal structure of short interval fillers", PLoS One Vol 10, e0141018. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0141018. [Crossref]
21. Hubbard T. L. (1995). "Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force", Psychon. Bull. Rev. Vol 2, 322338. [Crossref]
22. Hubbard T. L., Courtney J. R. (2010). "Cross-modal influences on representational momentum and representational gravity", Perception Vol 39, 851862. [Crossref]
23. Huber S., Krist H. (2004). "When is the ball going to hit the ground? Duration estimates, eye movements, and mental imagery of object motion", J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. Vol 30, 431444. [Crossref]
24. Johnson K. A., Bryan M., Polonowita K., Decroupet D., Coull J. T. (2016). "Isochronous sequential presentation helps children orient their attention in time", Front. Psychol. Vol 7, 1417. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01417.
25. Kawachi Y., Gyoba J. (2006). "A new response-time measure of object persistence in the tunnel effect", Acta. Psychol. Vol 123, 7390. [Crossref]
26. Kuroda T., Tomimatsu E., Grondin S., Miyazaki M. (2016). "Perceived empty duration between sounds of different lengths: possible relation with repetition and rhythmic grouping", Atten. Percept. Psychophys. Vol 78, 26782689. [Crossref]
27. Makin A. D. J., Lawson R., Bertamini M., Pickering J. (2014). "Auditory clicks distort perceived velocity but only when the system has to rely on extraretinal signals", Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Vol 67, 455473. [Crossref]
28. Meyer G. F., Wuerger S. M., Röhrbein F., Zetzsche C. (2005). "Low-level integration of auditory and visual motion signals requires spatial co-localisation", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 166, 538547. [Crossref]
29. Morein-Zamir S., Soto-Faraco S., Kingstone A. (2003). "Auditory capture of vision: examining temporal ventriloquism", Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. Vol 17, 154163. [Crossref]
30. Parrott S., Guzman-Martinez E., Ortega L., Grabowecky M., Huntington M. D., Suzuki S. (2015). "Direction of auditory pitch-change influences visual search for slope from graphs", Perception Vol 44, 764778. [Crossref]
31. Patel A. D., Iversen J. R., Chen Y., Repp B. H. (2005). "The influence of metricality and modality on synchronization with a beat", Exp. Brain Res. Vol 163, 226238. [Crossref]
32. Penton-Voak I. S., Edwards H., Percival A., Wearden J. H. (1996). "Speeding up an internal clock in humans? Effects of click trains on subjective duration", J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. Vol 22, 307320. [Crossref]
33. Poynter W. D. (1983). "Duration judgment and the segmentation of experience", Mem. Cognit. Vol 11, 7782. [Crossref]
34. Repp B. H., Penel A. (2002). "Auditory dominance in temporal processing: new evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences", J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. Vol 28, 10851099. [Crossref]
35. Repp B. H., Penel A. (2004). "Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms", Psychol. Res. Vol 68, 252270. [Crossref]
36. Roseboom W., Kawabe T., Nishida S. (2013). "The cross-modal double flash illusion depends on featural similarity between cross-modal inducers", Sci. Rep. Vol 3, 3437. DOI:10.1038/srep03437. [Crossref]
37. Sanabria D., Capizzi M., Correa Á. (2011). "Rhythms that speed you up", J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. Vol 37, 236244. [Crossref]
38. Sekuler R., Sekuler A. B., Lau R. (1997). "Sound alters visual motion perception", Nature Vol 385, 308. [Crossref]
39. Shams L., Kamitani Y., Shimojo S. (2002). "Visual illusion induced by sound", Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. Vol 14, 147152. [Crossref]
40. Sussman E., Gumenyuk V. (2005). "Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory", Neuroreport Vol 16, 15191523. [Crossref]
41. Thinès G., Costall A., Butterworth G. (Eds) (1991). Michotte’s Experimental Phenomenology of Perception. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, USA.
42. Treisman M., Faulkner A., Naish P. L. N., Brogan D. (1990). "The internal clock: evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency", Perception Vol 19, 705748. [Crossref]
43. Vroomen J., Keetels M., Gelder B. D., Bertelson P. (2004). "Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony", Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. Vol 22, 3235. [Crossref]
44. Watanabe K., Shimojo S. (2001). "When sound affects vision: effects of auditory grouping on visual motion perception", Psychol. Sci. Vol 12, 109116. [Crossref]
45. Wearden J. H., Norton R., Martin S., Montford-Bebb O. (2007). "Internal clock processes and the filled-duration illusion", J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. Vol 33, 716729. [Crossref]
46. Witten I. B., Knudsen E. I. (2005). "Why seeing is believing: merging auditory and visual worlds", Neuron Vol 48, 489496. [Crossref]
47. Wuerger S., Meyer G., Hofbauer M., Zetzsche C., Schill K. (2010). "Motion extrapolation of auditory–visual targets", Inf. Fusion Vol 11, 4550. [Crossref]
http://brill.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1163/22134808-00002592
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1163/22134808-00002592
2017-10-05
2017-11-23

Sign-in

Can't access your account?
  • Tools

  • Add to Favorites
  • Printable version
  • Email this page
  • Subscribe to ToC alert
  • Get permissions
  • Recommend to your library

    You must fill out fields marked with: *

    Librarian details
    Your details
    Why are you recommending this title?
    Select reason:
     
    Multisensory Research — Recommend this title to your library
  • Export citations
  • Key

  • Full access
  • Open Access
  • Partial/No accessInformation